2010
DOI: 10.1080/13670050902783528
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Interaction in bilingual phonological acquisition: evidence from phonetic inventories

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
90
0
4

Year Published

2011
2011
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 103 publications
(98 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
4
90
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Concerning acquisition of voicing, Fabiano-Smith and Barlow (2009) and Fabiano-Smith and Goldstein (2010a, b) noted 80-95% match across children for acquisition of voiceless stops in their groups of eight Spanish-speaking monolingual and bilinguals between ages 3;0 and 4;0. However, Macken and Barton (1980) noted relatively late acquisition of the voicing contrast in Spanish (not before age 5), possibly because of the prevoiced aspect of voiced stops (see also Macken, 1975).…”
Section: Acquisition Of Spanishmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Concerning acquisition of voicing, Fabiano-Smith and Barlow (2009) and Fabiano-Smith and Goldstein (2010a, b) noted 80-95% match across children for acquisition of voiceless stops in their groups of eight Spanish-speaking monolingual and bilinguals between ages 3;0 and 4;0. However, Macken and Barton (1980) noted relatively late acquisition of the voicing contrast in Spanish (not before age 5), possibly because of the prevoiced aspect of voiced stops (see also Macken, 1975).…”
Section: Acquisition Of Spanishmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…complexity of phonetic inventories), while at the same point in development a decelerated rate of acquisition was observed on others (e.g. phoneme accuracy; Fabiano-Smith & Barlow, 2010;Fabiano-Smith & Goldstein, 2010b;Lleó & Cortés, 2013;Lleó & Rakow, 2005). These findings harken back to the PRIMIR model in terms of how bilingual children might use information from one language to acquire structure in the other by making comparisons between their languages, or ''bootstrapping'' their language learning (Curtin et al, 2011, p. 496).…”
Section: Studies Of Speech Productionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Paradis and Genessee (1996), in particular, have proposed that different types of interaction might occur: (1) transfer, when elements specific to one language are used in productions of the other language; (2) deceleration, when interaction between the two languages interferes with, and thus slows down, the rate of acquisition; and (3) acceleration, when interaction between two languages aids in, and thus speeds up, the acquisition process. Although this study has only found evidence of acceleration as pointed out above, it is not ruled out that transfer, acceleration, and deceleration might indeed occur at the same time for different language aspects (see, for instance, Goldstein, 2010b, andBarlow, 2010). Evidently, there is need for more research that examines the issue of interdependence in groups of bilinguals and trilinguals instead that in individual children.…”
Section: The Importance Of Psychological and Social Factors In Infantmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…A variety of bilingual studies have shown that interaction between the two languages of bilingual children might aid in the acquisition process, allowing for a similar rate of development in bilinguals and monolinguals (Fabiano-Smith & Barlow, 2010;Fabiano-Smith & Goldstein, 2010b) and even for a faster rate of acquisition for some language constructs in bilinguals (see Gawlitzek-Maiwald &Tracy, 1996, for syntax andBunta, 2012, for phonology). In these studies, however, children were learning languages with a certain degree of structural or phonological overlap.…”
Section: The Importance Of Psychological and Social Factors In Infantmentioning
confidence: 99%