2018
DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2017.1344867
|View full text |Cite|
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Intentional preparation of auditory attention-switches: Explicit cueing and sequential switch-predictability

Abstract: In an auditory attention-switching paradigm, participants heard two simultaneously spoken number-words, each presented to one ear, and decided whether the target number was smaller or larger than 5 by pressing a left or right key. An instructional cue in each trial indicated which feature had to be used to identify the target number (e.g., female voice). Auditory attention-switch costs were found when this feature changed compared to when it repeated in two consecutive trials. Earlier studies employing this pa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
21
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
(102 reference statements)
0
21
0
Order By: Relevance
“…That the two voices were played dichotically in Koch et al's studies, with random ear assignment from trial to trial, may also have made it more difficult to prepare to attend to a male/female voice per se, as might the change in the voice selection criterion from ear in some blocks to gender in others (e.g. Seibold et al, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…That the two voices were played dichotically in Koch et al's studies, with random ear assignment from trial to trial, may also have made it more difficult to prepare to attend to a male/female voice per se, as might the change in the voice selection criterion from ear in some blocks to gender in others (e.g. Seibold et al, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The cue (height: 4 cm; average width: 8.2 cm) was presented in black color in the center of a white screen of a 22-inch computer monitor (LG 22MB65PM). We used a 2:1 cue-to-attention-criterion mapping, and the experimental program did not allow for direct cue repetitions to avoid visual cue repetition priming and isolate the effects of component repetition priming Lawo et al, 2014;Seibold, Nolden, Oberem, Fels, & Koch, 2017). In the speaker-sex cuing block, a female speaker was either cued by the German word Frau (German for Bwoman^) or the symbol ♀, whereas a male speaker was either cued by the word Mann (Bman^) or the symbol ♂.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When looking at previous studies on attention criterion switching, switch-specific effects of preparation time were rather inconsistent (see Koch et al, 2011; Lawo & Koch, 2015; Seibold et al, 2018b), even if attentional target selection occurred by ear (Lawo et al, 2014). Hence, it was not necessarily expected that a reduction of the attention criterion switch costs would be obtained.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…Despite the ability to switch quickly between task sets and their potential subsets, performance decrements occur when a switch of such a set is required, compared with when the task set can be maintained (for switch costs of the judgement, see Jersild, 1927; Rogers & Monsell, 1995; for reviews, see Kiesel et al, 2010; Koch et al, 2018). Also switches of the auditory selection criterion to identify an auditory target have been shown to result in costs (e.g., Koch, Lawo, Fels, & Vorländer, 2011; Lawo, Fels, Oberem, & Koch, 2014; Seibold, Nolden, Oberem, Fels, & Koch, 2018b). The same is true for switches of the visual attention criterion for target selection (see Logan, 2005; Longman, Lavric, & Monsell, 2016; Longman, Lavric, Munteanu, & Monsell, 2014).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation