2001
DOI: 10.1080/09541440126215
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Inhibitory control over the present and the past

Abstract: From the perspective of the Hasher, Zacks, and May (1999) inhibitory framework, optimal performance occurs only when there is control over nonrelevant information. Relative to a current, goal-directed task, there are at least two potential sources of nonrelevant information that need to be controlled. The rst is no longer relevant information. Such information would include, for example, a previous topic of conversation, or, in our work, a previous list of materials presented for study and recall. The second s… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
42
0

Year Published

2001
2001
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
4

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 69 publications
(45 citation statements)
references
References 65 publications
3
42
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, even when subjects performed as many as 20 retrieval practice trials on the dominant sense, little retrieval-induced forgetting was observed. Taken together these results argue against associative blocking accounts of retrieval-induced forgetting, but are consistent with idea that this phenomenon depends on the need to override prepotent memories, as would be expected if inhibitory processes are recruited to suppress those memories (see Conway & Engle, 1994 for a related discussion of the role of inhibitory processes in resolving interference in memory span tasks; see also, Lustig, Hasher, & Toney, 2001, for a recent review of work on inhibitory processes in cognitive aging).…”
Section: Interference Dependencesupporting
confidence: 76%
“…Thus, even when subjects performed as many as 20 retrieval practice trials on the dominant sense, little retrieval-induced forgetting was observed. Taken together these results argue against associative blocking accounts of retrieval-induced forgetting, but are consistent with idea that this phenomenon depends on the need to override prepotent memories, as would be expected if inhibitory processes are recruited to suppress those memories (see Conway & Engle, 1994 for a related discussion of the role of inhibitory processes in resolving interference in memory span tasks; see also, Lustig, Hasher, & Toney, 2001, for a recent review of work on inhibitory processes in cognitive aging).…”
Section: Interference Dependencesupporting
confidence: 76%
“…For example, the PSWQ-A may reduce respondent confusion as it pertains to answering (reversed) items on self-report measures of worry. The absence of reversed items on this scale may assist in more reliably assessing cognitively impaired or otherwise disabled individuals, as well as older adults who may be more susceptible to internal or external distraction and less capable of manipulating (reversed) information (Hasher & Zacks, 1988;Lustig, Hasher, & Tonev, 2001). The PSWQ-A also may be particularly useful in primary care settings.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Hasher and Zacks (1988) proposed the inhibitory deficit theory of cognitive aging, which tries to explain a wide range of age-related difficulties in cognitive functioning by a deficit in the inhibitory control system. This framework aims to explain the difficulties experienced by older individuals find at restraining taskirrelevant information as well as prepotent responses, and getting rid of irrelevant information from working memory (Hasher and Zacks 1988;Hasher et al 2001;Lustig et al 2001).…”
Section: Attentional Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%