2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.013
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Inflectional spelling deficits in developmental dyslexia

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

6
31
3
5

Year Published

2013
2013
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(45 citation statements)
references
References 97 publications
6
31
3
5
Order By: Relevance
“…Understanding that the “end state” of the written production system involves orthographic representations and processes sensitive to the morphological structure of words is relevant for literacy instruction and rehabilitation. Considerable research has examined the relationship between general morphological skills and literacy development (Nagy, Beninger & Abbott, 2006), while less has specifically examined the development of morpho-orthography (but see, Egan & Tainturier, 2011; Treiman & Cassar, 1996). Learning and rehabilitation experiences that target orthographic morphological structures and processes may contribute to developing the type and level of expertise of the adult writer.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Understanding that the “end state” of the written production system involves orthographic representations and processes sensitive to the morphological structure of words is relevant for literacy instruction and rehabilitation. Considerable research has examined the relationship between general morphological skills and literacy development (Nagy, Beninger & Abbott, 2006), while less has specifically examined the development of morpho-orthography (but see, Egan & Tainturier, 2011; Treiman & Cassar, 1996). Learning and rehabilitation experiences that target orthographic morphological structures and processes may contribute to developing the type and level of expertise of the adult writer.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the English orthography, there are at least five relatively distinct sources of knowledge that contribute to accurate spelling (e.g. Bourassa & Treiman, 2001;Egan & Tainturier, 2011;Joshi, Treiman, Carreker, & Moats, 2008;Kemp, Parrila, & Kirby, 2009;Moats, 2009): (1) knowing how to translate sounds into letters (spelling phonics, or "non-lexical spelling"), (2) orthographic memory (sight word knowledge, or "lexical spelling"), (3) semantic principles, (4) morphology and (5) etymology.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To examine whether children with dyslexia show atypical morphological processing given their level of literacy skills, a better comparison is against younger, reading-ability-matched peers. For the most part, children with dyslexia perform similarly to reading-ability-matched children on oral measures of morphological awareness (Casalis et al, 2004;Egan & Pring, 2004;Egan & Tainturier, 2011;Robertson, Joanisse, Desroches, & Terry, 2012;Tsesmeli & Seymour, 2006). However, the picture for morphological processing is less clear.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the picture for morphological processing is less clear. Some have found weaknesses in morphological spelling (Carlisle, 1987;Egan & Pring, 2004;Egan & Tainturier, 2011;Hauerwas & Walker, 2003;Tsesmeli & Seymour, 2006) whereas others have found no difference compared to ability matches (Bourassa, Treiman, & Kessler, 2006;. However, it is worth noting that very few studies have examined morphological processing in dyslexic children using a reading-ability-match design (Deacon et al, in press).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation