2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.08.015
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Inferior frontal oscillations reveal visuo-motor matching for actions and speech: evidence from human intracranial recordings

Abstract: The neural correspondence between the systems responsible for the execution and recognition of actions has been suggested both in humans and non-human primates. Apart from being a key region of this visuo-motor observation-execution matching (OEM) system, the human inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) is also important for speech production. The functional overlap of visuo-motor OEM and speech, together with the phylogenetic history of the IFG as a motor area, has led to the idea that speech function has evolved from … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
8
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

5
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 113 publications
1
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In other words, in pathological conditions affecting the acquirement of the information required to correctly represent the body, we are able to switch strategy in order to use a different reference frame (e.g., visuospatial) and accommodate ecologically appropriate adaptations, e.g., visually guided movements (Rothwell et al, 1982). This interpretation is in line with the evidence suggesting that movement representations are largely multimodal (Halje et al, 2015) and that the interplay between these different representations can change in the case of missing (Curtze et al, 2010), disconnected (Fiori et al, 2014) or misrepresented limbs (Daprati et al, 2010).…”
Section: Modeling Sensory-motor Integrationsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…In other words, in pathological conditions affecting the acquirement of the information required to correctly represent the body, we are able to switch strategy in order to use a different reference frame (e.g., visuospatial) and accommodate ecologically appropriate adaptations, e.g., visually guided movements (Rothwell et al, 1982). This interpretation is in line with the evidence suggesting that movement representations are largely multimodal (Halje et al, 2015) and that the interplay between these different representations can change in the case of missing (Curtze et al, 2010), disconnected (Fiori et al, 2014) or misrepresented limbs (Daprati et al, 2010).…”
Section: Modeling Sensory-motor Integrationsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…In this vein, motor imagery (an active mental rehearsal of movements without physical execution 12 ) is an ideal cognitive task. Cerebral representations of actions are largely multimodal 13 and imagined and executed movements show proportional timing (longer movements require longer time to be imagined) 14 and kinematic configuration (anatomically awkward movements are more difficult to imagine) 15 . In addition, despite specific differences 16 , they engage partially overlapping brain networks 17 18 19 .…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similarly, handwriting improves after haptic (not visual) exploration of letters even at younger age (Bara et al, 2004), and is associated with better visual recognition of letters with respect to typing (Longcamp et al, 2005) and with better reading in general (Labat et al, 2010). At the neural level, in addition to the anatomo-functional overlap of brain regions activated by "seeing" and "doing" movements (Halje et al, 2015), already in 9-month-old children the motor components of the brain activity associated to observation of reaching actions occur earlier than the associated visual components (Southgate et al, 2009). This supports the existence of visuo-motor anticipation mechanisms based on experiencedriven action understanding (Southgate et al, 2010).…”
Section: Eye-hand Coordinationmentioning
confidence: 98%