2000
DOI: 10.1080/13506280050144399
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Infants' use of gaze direction to cue attention: The importance of perceived motion

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

10
183
1

Year Published

2002
2002
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
4
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 236 publications
(197 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
10
183
1
Order By: Relevance
“…One possible explanation of this discrepancy relies upon the differences between the two paradigms. First, gaze processing might have been facilitated in Vuilleumier's study by the face context; second, attention orienting was triggered by moving eyes that are a much stronger cue compared with static eyes because they also convey a motion cue, as demonstrated in infants (Farroni, Johnson, Brockbank, & Simion, 2000). The crucial finding, however, is that the effect of arrow cues in neglect patients consistently resembled patterns previously described as characterizing peripheral noninformative cues (for a thorough review, see Losier & Klein, 2001).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 43%
“…One possible explanation of this discrepancy relies upon the differences between the two paradigms. First, gaze processing might have been facilitated in Vuilleumier's study by the face context; second, attention orienting was triggered by moving eyes that are a much stronger cue compared with static eyes because they also convey a motion cue, as demonstrated in infants (Farroni, Johnson, Brockbank, & Simion, 2000). The crucial finding, however, is that the effect of arrow cues in neglect patients consistently resembled patterns previously described as characterizing peripheral noninformative cues (for a thorough review, see Losier & Klein, 2001).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 43%
“…First, Ristic, Friesen, and Kingstone (2002) found that preschool children showed greater orienting effects than adults in response to nonpredictive gaze-direction cues, despite the fact that young children are thought to be poor at volitional orienting (Brodeur, Trick, & Enns, 1997). Second, Hood, Willen, and Driver (1998) found that infants were faster to make saccades to peripheral targets that were cued nonpredictively by the gaze direction of a central face and concluded that gaze-triggered orienting is in place very early in development (but see Farroni, Johnson, Brockbank, & Simion, 2000, for an alternative explanation). Third, in their split-brain patient study, Kingstone et al (2000) found that although only the cortical hemisphere specialized for face and gaze processing oriented reflexively in response to nonpredictive gaze cues, both hemispheres oriented volitionally in response to predictive gaze cues.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the case of the gaze-cueing effect, the role of more complex dynamic stimuli is subtle. In infants, motion appears to be necessary for gaze orienting (Farroni, Johnson, Brockbank, & Simion, 2000). Nonetheless, neither photographic faces nor dynamic movement in gaze cues have been shown to increase the magnitude of the gaze cueing effect with adults (Hietanen & Leppänen, 2003).…”
Section: Attending To Action In Interactive Contextsmentioning
confidence: 99%