2007
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2007.07.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Infant manual exploration of composite substrates

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4

Citation Types

2
38
0
1

Year Published

2010
2010
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 62 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
2
38
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In fact three weeks of enhanced postural training led to improved reaching, mouthing, and fingering of objects in 2- to 5-month-old TD infants (Lobo and Galloway, 2008). Moreover, object exploration skills have implications for other forms of development such as perceptual (Needham, 2000; Bhat and Galloway, 2006; Lobo and Galloway, 2008; Koterba et al, 2012), social communication (Meltzoff, 1995; Fagan and Iverson, 2007; Iverson et al, 2007), and cognitive development (Caruso, 1993; Bourgeois et al, 2005; Fontenelle et al, 2007). In terms of perceptual development, infants learn various object properties such as texture, shape, size, color, and sound while exploring objects (Ruff, 1984, 1986; Palmer, 1989; Rochat, 1989).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In fact three weeks of enhanced postural training led to improved reaching, mouthing, and fingering of objects in 2- to 5-month-old TD infants (Lobo and Galloway, 2008). Moreover, object exploration skills have implications for other forms of development such as perceptual (Needham, 2000; Bhat and Galloway, 2006; Lobo and Galloway, 2008; Koterba et al, 2012), social communication (Meltzoff, 1995; Fagan and Iverson, 2007; Iverson et al, 2007), and cognitive development (Caruso, 1993; Bourgeois et al, 2005; Fontenelle et al, 2007). In terms of perceptual development, infants learn various object properties such as texture, shape, size, color, and sound while exploring objects (Ruff, 1984, 1986; Palmer, 1989; Rochat, 1989).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Rhythmic movements involve for example repeated waving, scratching, petting, wiping, hitting (with or without another object), turning (e.g. to perceive objects from different perspectives), etc., and allow infants to discover the functioning of their own body, the structure of the world, and the potential effects of their actions on it [8,10,11,12]. Rhythmic movements are probably so important because, being repetitive, they allow infants to acquire multiple sample data needed to develop motor skills despite the high noise of early behaviour.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Importantly, the development of manipulation skills implies a gradual passage from random exploratory rhythmic movements to fu nctional movements that produce interesting, relevant, or useful effects on the environment. These functional movements can be either rhythmic or discrete [10,13,14]. The literature on the development of manipulation skills lacks in models and hypotheses to investigate and to frame mechanisms and processes that lead to the progressive development of rhythmic movements.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, it could be shown that infants squeeze soft objects more than rigid ones or that they scratch objects more, when they are textured, when compared with non-textured objects (e.g. Bourgeois, Khawar, Neal, & Lockman, 2005;Bushnell & Boudreau, 1998;Fontanelle, Kahrs, Neal, Newton, & Lockman, 2007;Palmer, 1989;Rochat, 1987).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%