2018
DOI: 10.1111/desc.12779
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Infant gaze following depends on communicative signals: An eye‐tracking study of 5‐ to 7‐month‐olds in Vanuatu

Abstract: Gaze is considered a crucial component of early communication between an infant and her caregiver. When communicatively addressed, infants respond aptly to others’ gaze by following its direction. However, experience with face‐to‐face contact varies across cultures, begging the question whether infants’ competencies in receiving others’ communicative gaze signals are universal or culturally specific . We used eye‐tracking to assess gaze‐following responses of 5‐ to 7‐month olds in Vanuatu, where face‐to‐face p… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

3
60
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(63 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
(136 reference statements)
3
60
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Based on this result, Csibra (2010) suggested that gaze following only occurs in the narrow context of ostensive cues early in life. Recently, Hernik and Broesch (2019) replicated this finding in 5-to 7-month-old infants developing in Vanuatu community, suggesting that the phenomenon is not fully dependent on a Western style of parenting. However, other studies showed conflicting evidence.…”
Section: Communicative Cuesmentioning
confidence: 74%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Based on this result, Csibra (2010) suggested that gaze following only occurs in the narrow context of ostensive cues early in life. Recently, Hernik and Broesch (2019) replicated this finding in 5-to 7-month-old infants developing in Vanuatu community, suggesting that the phenomenon is not fully dependent on a Western style of parenting. However, other studies showed conflicting evidence.…”
Section: Communicative Cuesmentioning
confidence: 74%
“…However, such a viewpoint contradicts Senju and Csibra (2008) which did not observe gaze following in the situation with an attention-grabbing animation on the model's face (see also Hernik and Broesch, 2019). In addition, as discussed above, Gredebäck et al (2018) failed to show the effect of attentiongrabbing cue on gaze following.…”
Section: Infant Attentionmentioning
confidence: 86%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Observed shifts of gaze‐direction bias covert visual attention (gaze‐cueing) in very young human infants and possibly newborns (Farroni, Massaccesi, Pividori & Johnson, 2004). They elicit overt shift of observer’s own gaze (gaze‐following) in infants as young as 5–6 months of age, raised in diverse cultural contexts (Gredebäck, Astor, & Fawcett, 2018; Gredebäck, Astor, & Fawcett, 2008; Hernik & Broesch, 2019; Senju & Csibra, 2008). Gaze‐cueing and gaze‐following may be best expressed in response to dynamic and communicative gaze‐signals in infants (Farroni, Johnson, Brockbank, & Simion, 2000; Gredebäck et al, 2018; Hernik & Broesch, 2019; Senju & Csibra, 2008), although such signals are not required for adults (Friesen & Kingstone, 1998).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Whether or not infants are able to gaze follow in a given study seems to be affected not only by age but also whether stimulus presentation is live or computerized, whether there is perceived movement, whether a change in eye gaze direction is accompanied by a head turn, whether a communicative or attention directing cue is used before the gaze shift, and what measure is used (for a review, see [10]). Eye-tracking studies of gaze following show some evidence of gaze following from 5 months of age [11], [12], [13], [14], including in non-WEIRD (Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic) populations [15].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%