2014
DOI: 10.1111/desc.12219
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Individual differences in the shape bias in preschool children with specific language impairment and typical language development: theoretical and clinical implications

Abstract: We investigated whether preschool children with specific language impairment (SLI) exhibit the shape bias in word learning: the bias to generalize based on shape rather than size, color, or texture in an object naming context ('This is a wek; find another wek') but not in a non-naming similarity classification context ('See this? Which one goes with this one?'). Fifty-four preschool children (16 with SLI, 16 children with typical language [TL] in an equated control group, and 22 additional children with TL inc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

1
53
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 49 publications
(55 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
1
53
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, it is possible that more "advanced" babies come to more informatively manipulate objects to create more variability in objects' images and then also through some independent pathway build larger vocabularies. There are also increasing indicators that children who are slow to grow their vocabularies show delays or disruptions in visual processes (Behrmann, Thomas, & Humphreys, 2006;Collisson, Grela, Spaulding, Rueckl, & Magnuson, 2015;Jones & Smith, 2005). The importance of a deeper understanding of the visual information-and the role of infant manipulations in generating that information-extends both to fundamental questions about visual object recognition and to understanding early object name learning.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, it is possible that more "advanced" babies come to more informatively manipulate objects to create more variability in objects' images and then also through some independent pathway build larger vocabularies. There are also increasing indicators that children who are slow to grow their vocabularies show delays or disruptions in visual processes (Behrmann, Thomas, & Humphreys, 2006;Collisson, Grela, Spaulding, Rueckl, & Magnuson, 2015;Jones & Smith, 2005). The importance of a deeper understanding of the visual information-and the role of infant manipulations in generating that information-extends both to fundamental questions about visual object recognition and to understanding early object name learning.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The majority of research on shape bias has been experimental in nature (e.g., Booth, Waxman, & Huang, ; Collisson, Grela, Spaulding, Rueckl, & Magnuson, ; Graham & Diesendruck, ). This research strongly supports a link between shape bias and word learning.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, 2‐ and 3‐year‐old late talkers extend the name of a novel solid object to other objects that match it in shape to a lesser extent than an age‐matched typically developing control group (Jones, ). Similarly, preschool children with specific language impairment show no shape bias compared with age‐ and SES‐matched typically developing controls (Collisson, Grela, Spaulding, Rueckl, & Magnuson, ). In fact, in one study, almost half of the late talkers systematically extended the novel name of a solid object to others matching in texture rather than shape (Jones, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%