2000
DOI: 10.1037/1040-3590.12.4.402
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Incremental validity of WISC–III profile elevation, scatter, and shape information for predicting reading and math achievement.

Abstract: The use of cognitive subtest profiles to hypothesize about children's learning strengths and weaknesses implicitly assumes that subtest profiles are predictive of academic performance. To test this assumption, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition (WISC-III) subtest profiles were decomposed into elevation, scatter, and shape components and sequentially regressed onto reading and math achievement scores for 1,118 nonexceptional and 538 exceptional students. Profile elevation was statistically a… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
48
2
5

Year Published

2005
2005
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 52 publications
(60 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
5
48
2
5
Order By: Relevance
“…If external criteria such as academic achievement in reading, mathematics, and writing are significantly predicted by the first-order or CHC Stratum II factors (verbal-crystallized and nonverbal-fluid-visual dimensions) after accounting for prediction by the higher order CHC Stratum III factor (general intelligence), then those lower order dimensions may be of importance for predictive and interpretive purposes. Incremental validity studies of more "comprehensive" measures of intelligence, however, have not supported many first-order factors or CHC Stratum II factor-based scores (Glutting, Youngstrom, et al, 1997;Kahana et al, 2002;Konold, 1999;Ree & Earles, 1991;Ree et al, 1994;Watkins & Glutting, 2000;Watkins et al, 2007;Youngstrom et al, 1999). It may be that even the longer, more "comprehensive" measures of intelligence will require additional subtests to capture sufficient variance at the first-order or CHC Stratum II level to provide additional meaningful prediction of academic achievement beyond the second-order CHC Stratum III general intelligence factor.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…If external criteria such as academic achievement in reading, mathematics, and writing are significantly predicted by the first-order or CHC Stratum II factors (verbal-crystallized and nonverbal-fluid-visual dimensions) after accounting for prediction by the higher order CHC Stratum III factor (general intelligence), then those lower order dimensions may be of importance for predictive and interpretive purposes. Incremental validity studies of more "comprehensive" measures of intelligence, however, have not supported many first-order factors or CHC Stratum II factor-based scores (Glutting, Youngstrom, et al, 1997;Kahana et al, 2002;Konold, 1999;Ree & Earles, 1991;Ree et al, 1994;Watkins & Glutting, 2000;Watkins et al, 2007;Youngstrom et al, 1999). It may be that even the longer, more "comprehensive" measures of intelligence will require additional subtests to capture sufficient variance at the first-order or CHC Stratum II level to provide additional meaningful prediction of academic achievement beyond the second-order CHC Stratum III general intelligence factor.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although some have argued that clinical assessments require that a "comprehensive" battery of intelligence tests be administered to fully understand the nature of performance deficits in the context of individual profiles, research on various subtest analyses (i.e., subtest strengths and weaknesses or unique profiles) reveals them to lack sufficient reliability and validity (Canivez & Watkins, 1998, 1999Glutting, McDermott, Konold, Snelbaker, & Watkins, 1998;Glutting, McDermott, Watkins, Kush, & Konold, 1997;Macmann & Barnett, 1997;McDermott, Fantuzzo, Glutting, Watkins, & Baggaley, 1992;Watkins & Canivez, 2004). The incremental validity (Haynes & Lench, 2003;Hunsley, 2003;Hunsley & Meyer, 2003) of factor-based scores of more "comprehensive" measures of intelligence has been questioned and found lacking (Glutting, Youngstrom, Ward, Ward, & Hale, 1997;Kahana, Youngstrom, & Glutting, 2002;Konold, 1999;Ree & Earles, 1991;Ree, Earles, & Treachout, 1994;Watkins & Glutting, 2000;Watkins, Glutting, & Lei, 2007;Youngstrom, Kogos, & Glutting, 1999). When estimating general intellectual functioning without regard to examining subtest performance, patterns, or profiles, intelligence tests with fewer subtests may provide more time-and costeffective yet valid assessment.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The relation between early, overall cognitive performance (at or before kindergarten) and later school achievement is stronger for reading skills than it is for math skills (Stevenson & Newman, 1986); so assessments of broad skills alone, such as IQ testing, are not useful for identifying children with specific MLD. The pattern or scatter of IQ subtest scores is also not predictive of math (or reading) achievement (Watkins & Glutting, 2000), despite use of sub-test scores to define specific syndromes in clinical and school settings (as reviewed by Watkins, Kush, & Glutting, 1997). Thus, to identify MLD, it is necessary to identify behavioral criteria that are not captured by broad-level achievement or intellectual testing.…”
Section: Predicting Poor Math Achievementmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, Naglieri, Goldstein, Iseman and Schwebach (2003) found no differences on the WISC-III between children with ADHD and children with Anxiety /Depression. Other studies provided little or no support for the use of WISC-III subtest pattern in the diagnosis of LD (Daley & Nagle, 1996;Watkins, 1999;Watkins & Glutting, 2000). Also, the SCAD and ACID profiles were not found to be valid diagnostic indicators or important predictors of academic achievement .…”
Section: Predictive Validity and Clinical Utility Of The American Wismentioning
confidence: 99%