2021
DOI: 10.1101/2021.06.16.448743
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Increase of both bottom-up and top-down attentional processes in high dream recallers

Abstract: Event-related potentials (ERPs) associated with the involuntary orientation of (bottom-up) attention towards an unexpected sound are of larger amplitude in high dream recallers (HR) than in low dream recallers (LR) during passive listening, suggesting different attentional functioning. We measured bottom-up and top-down attentional performance and their cerebral correlates in 18 HR (11 women, age = 22.7 +/- 4.1 years, dream recall frequency = 5.3 +/- 1.3 days with a dream recall per week) and 19 LR (10 women, … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
1
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
2

Relationship

0
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(1 citation statement)
references
References 41 publications
(70 reference statements)
0
1
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However the results also point to differences between working memory and selective attention processes, as revealed by slightly different patterns of results in various participants groups: on the one hand, musicians exhibit greatly enhanced working memory abilities for musical sequences but only limited advantages if any for selective attention compared to non-musicians (Blain, Talamini, et al, 2022), and on the other hand, participants with high dream recall frequency are more sensitive to the presence of difficult-to-filter-out distractors than participants with a low dream recall frequency, whereas the impact of the memory task difficulty manipulation was similar is these two participant groups (Blain, de la Chapelle, et al, 2022). These between-groups differences reveal that attention and working memory are differently sensitive to expertise effects (for the effect of musical expertise on auditory working memory see for example George & Coch, 2011;Talamini et al, 2016Talamini et al, , 2017Talamini et al, , 2022 and psychophysiological traits (for the effect of such traits on auditory attention, see Ruby et al, 2013;Eichenlaub, Bertrand, et al, 2014;Eichenlaub, Nicolas, et al, 2014;Vallat et al, 2020Vallat et al, , 2022Ruby et al, 2022). Overall, behavioral results obtained so far with the MEMAT paradigm revealed both shared and separated processes subtending auditory selective attention and working memory.…”
Section: The Current Studymentioning
confidence: 94%
“…However the results also point to differences between working memory and selective attention processes, as revealed by slightly different patterns of results in various participants groups: on the one hand, musicians exhibit greatly enhanced working memory abilities for musical sequences but only limited advantages if any for selective attention compared to non-musicians (Blain, Talamini, et al, 2022), and on the other hand, participants with high dream recall frequency are more sensitive to the presence of difficult-to-filter-out distractors than participants with a low dream recall frequency, whereas the impact of the memory task difficulty manipulation was similar is these two participant groups (Blain, de la Chapelle, et al, 2022). These between-groups differences reveal that attention and working memory are differently sensitive to expertise effects (for the effect of musical expertise on auditory working memory see for example George & Coch, 2011;Talamini et al, 2016Talamini et al, , 2017Talamini et al, , 2022 and psychophysiological traits (for the effect of such traits on auditory attention, see Ruby et al, 2013;Eichenlaub, Bertrand, et al, 2014;Eichenlaub, Nicolas, et al, 2014;Vallat et al, 2020Vallat et al, , 2022Ruby et al, 2022). Overall, behavioral results obtained so far with the MEMAT paradigm revealed both shared and separated processes subtending auditory selective attention and working memory.…”
Section: The Current Studymentioning
confidence: 94%