2005
DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3318
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Inadvertent social information in breeding site selection of natal dispersing birds

Abstract: Several species use the number of young produced as public information (PI) to assess breeding site quality. PI is inaccessible for synchronously breeding birds because nests are empty by the time the young can collect this information. We investigate if location cues are the next best source of inadvertent social information (ISI) used by young prospectors during breeding site choice. We experimentally deployed ISI as decoys and song playbacks of breeding males in suitable and sub-optimal habitats during pre-… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

6
193
2
1

Year Published

2007
2007
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 161 publications
(203 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
(87 reference statements)
6
193
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Though previous studies have succeeded in manipulating immigration and emigration, the actual cues used in habitat selection have remained poorly known Nocera et al 2006;Parejo et al 2007). Fledgling condition (e.g.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Though previous studies have succeeded in manipulating immigration and emigration, the actual cues used in habitat selection have remained poorly known Nocera et al 2006;Parejo et al 2007). Fledgling condition (e.g.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a field experiment investigating breeding habitat choice of first-time breeding natal dispersing birds of the solitary Nelson's sharp-tailed sparrow (Ammodramus nelsoni ) and social bobolink (Dolichonyx oryzivorus), Nocera et al [46] provide strong evidence that only young inexperienced bobolinks rely on social information (location cues) provided by visual models and audio playbacks of adult males to choose a breeding habitat. Interestingly, they report that erroneous social information (i.e.…”
Section: Using Incorrect Social Informationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…when audio playbacks and visual models were played and placed in suboptimal habitats) induces young bobolinks to settle in and even defend these suboptimal territories the following spring. Nocera et al's [46] work is compelling because of the experimental technique that was adopted: audio playback. In such a situation, the experimenter controls the quality of the social information and as a result provides a convincing demonstration of the potential effect of false social information use directly and in field conditions.…”
Section: Using Incorrect Social Informationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, breeding season length may influence responsiveness, where species with very short breeding windows (i.e., explosive breeders) might use conspecific cues to quickly locate breeding habitat [18]. Breeding pond habitat use may also influence social information use, where species breeding in temporally or spatially unpredictable ponds might be more likely to exploit conspecific cues than those breeding in reliable water bodies (i.e., permanent ponds; 23).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Anurans typically have few opportunities to breed and, when they do, produce large numbers of young. Seemingly, such characteristics might predispose anurans to conspecific attraction and using conspecific calls to locate breeding locations [18], yet field evidence on the topic is scarce [19,20]. Several decades ago, Oldham [21] found that green frogs (Lithobates clamitans) transplanted to a foreign location were more likely to orient toward conspecific calls than were local frogs, and, more recently, James et al [22] found that conspecific call playbacks influenced the within-pond distributions of green and golden bell frogs (Litoria aurea).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%