2012
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00367
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

In Support of a Distinction between Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Control: A Review of the Literature on Proportion Congruent Effects

Abstract: Cognitive control is by now a large umbrella term referring collectively to multiple processes that plan and coordinate actions to meet task goals. A common feature of paradigms that engage cognitive control is the task requirement to select relevant information despite a habitual tendency (or bias) to select goal-irrelevant information. At least since the 1970s, researchers have employed proportion congruent (PC) manipulations to experimentally establish selection biases and evaluate the mechanisms used to co… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

25
323
2
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 294 publications
(351 citation statements)
references
References 95 publications
(201 reference statements)
25
323
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…By this alternative view, the requirement to respond to the word BLUE in red could result in learning that favours processing of the colour dimension relative to the word dimension for that particular item, but not for other items. This idea gained initial support from findings of itemspecific proportion congruency effects in the Stroop task (Jacoby, Lindsay, & Hessels, 2003;see also Blais, Robidoux, Risko, & Besner, 2007;Bugg, Jacoby, & Toth, 2008; for a review see Bugg & Crump, 2012). Jacoby et al (2003) manipulated the relative proportions of congruent and incongruent items separately for different sets of Stroop items.…”
Section: Selective Attention and Episodic Specificitymentioning
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…By this alternative view, the requirement to respond to the word BLUE in red could result in learning that favours processing of the colour dimension relative to the word dimension for that particular item, but not for other items. This idea gained initial support from findings of itemspecific proportion congruency effects in the Stroop task (Jacoby, Lindsay, & Hessels, 2003;see also Blais, Robidoux, Risko, & Besner, 2007;Bugg, Jacoby, & Toth, 2008; for a review see Bugg & Crump, 2012). Jacoby et al (2003) manipulated the relative proportions of congruent and incongruent items separately for different sets of Stroop items.…”
Section: Selective Attention and Episodic Specificitymentioning
confidence: 92%
“…One interpretation of sequential congruency effects is that they reflect adaptations in cognitive control processes (Botvinick et al, 2001; but see Hommel, Proctor, & Vu, 2004;Mayr, Awh, & Laurey, 2003 for 4 incongruent trial transitions are more frequent in blocks with a high proportion of incongruent trials, carryover of preparation from one incongruent trial to the next also offers an explanation of proportion congruent effects (smaller congruency effects in blocks with a relatively high proportion of incongruent trials) in Stroop and Stroop-like tasks (Logan & Zbrodoff, 1979;Lowe & Mitterer, 1982;Tzelgov, Henik, & Berger, 1992; for a review see Bugg & Crump, 2012).…”
Section: Selective Attention and Episodic Specificitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In other conflict paradigms (e.g., Stroop, flanker tasks), increasing the proportion of conflict trials is thought to engage voluntary and global proactive control mechanisms that are sustained across trials (for a review, see Bugg & Crump, 2012). However, control might also be triggered more locally by the presence of a distractor on a preceding trial through a mechanism of conflict adaptation (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001), which of course occurs more often in the high frequency condition.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These frequency effects are analogous to those seen in other types of conflict tasks, such as traditional Stroop or flanker tasks, in which the distracting information shares features with the target and so can create response conflict. In such tasks, performance improves when the proportion of conflict trials increases -an effect that has been attributed to increased cognitive control when conflicts are expected (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001;Dishon-Berkovits & Algom, 2000; see reviews in Bugg, 2012;Bugg & Crump, 2012). The Dual Mechanisms of Control (DMC) model (Braver, 2012;Braver, Gray & Burgess, 2007;Braver, Paxton, Locke, & Barch, 2009) provides a useful framework in which to interpret the effect of distractor frequency on cognitive control.…”
Section: Cognitive Control Of Non-emotional Distractionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nouns preceded the adjectives, in accordance with the Hebrew syntax (examples in this manuscript are provided in the reverse order in favor of fluency and compatibility with the English syntax). List-based distinctiveness was parametrically modulated via the proportion of the familiar and novel stimuli in different experimental lists (Icht, Mama, & Algom, 2014; see also list-wise proportion manipulation in attention and semantic priming; Blais, Harris, Sinanian, & Bunge, 2015;Bugg & Crump, 2012;Tweedy, Lapinski, & Schvaneveldt, 1977). A subsequent surprise noun recognition test was utilized to examine memory effects for items encoded with a familiar or a novel adjective.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%