2020
DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/vakm8
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Implicit and explicit learning of socio-emotional information in a dynamic system control task

Abstract: Unconscious or, implicit learning (IL) is often described as being instrumental to human social functioning. However, most of the available IL tasks have limited external validity; they use surface stimuli that are not socially relevant. Additionally, the way in which participants exchange information within most of the available tasks departs from the way in which information is being exchanged in real-life social situations. In this study, we report the validation of a novel task, inspired from Broadbent et… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
2
1
1
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This method has been most commonly applied in AGL, where it has shown that participants acquire, not only conscious, but also unconscious (structural) knowledge of the grammars, since they respond accurately when they rely on unconscious structural knowledge (Guess, Intuition, Familiarity) (e.g., Dienes & Scott, 2005;Norman & Price, 2012;Norman et al, 2016Norman et al, , 2019Scott & Dienes, 2008. Similar results with this method have been found in evaluative conditioning (Jurchis et al, 2020;Waroquier et al, 2020), implicit sequence learning (Fu et al, 2012(Fu et al, , 2018Zhang & Liu, 2021), language learning (e.g., Paciorek & Williams, 2015;Zhao et al, 2021), symmetry learning (Jiang et al, 2012;Ling et al, 2018), learning of conjunctive rules (Neil & Higham, 2012, or implicit social learning (Costea, 2018;Costea et al, 2020).…”
Section: Figurementioning
confidence: 69%
“…This method has been most commonly applied in AGL, where it has shown that participants acquire, not only conscious, but also unconscious (structural) knowledge of the grammars, since they respond accurately when they rely on unconscious structural knowledge (Guess, Intuition, Familiarity) (e.g., Dienes & Scott, 2005;Norman & Price, 2012;Norman et al, 2016Norman et al, , 2019Scott & Dienes, 2008. Similar results with this method have been found in evaluative conditioning (Jurchis et al, 2020;Waroquier et al, 2020), implicit sequence learning (Fu et al, 2012(Fu et al, , 2018Zhang & Liu, 2021), language learning (e.g., Paciorek & Williams, 2015;Zhao et al, 2021), symmetry learning (Jiang et al, 2012;Ling et al, 2018), learning of conjunctive rules (Neil & Higham, 2012, or implicit social learning (Costea, 2018;Costea et al, 2020).…”
Section: Figurementioning
confidence: 69%
“…This method has been most commonly applied in AGL, where it has shown that participants acquire, not only conscious, but also unconscious (structural) knowledge of the grammars, since they respond accurately when they rely on unconscious structural knowledge (Guess, Intuition, Familiarity) (e.g., Dienes & Scott, 2005;Norman & Price, 2012;Norman et al, 2016Norman et al, , 2019Scott & Dienes, 2008. Similar results with this method have been found in evaluative conditioning (Jurchis et al, 2020;Waroquier et al, 2020), implicit sequence learning (Fu et al, 2012(Fu et al, , 2018Zhang & Liu, 2021), language learning (e.g., Paciorek & Williams, 2015;Zhao et al, 2021), symmetry learning (Jiang et al, 2012;Ling et al, 2018), learning of conjunctive rules (Neil & Higham, 2012, or implicit social learning (Costea, 2018;Costea et al, 2020).…”
Section: Figurementioning
confidence: 75%