2014
DOI: 10.1521/jscp.2014.33.4.319
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Impaired Emotional Clarity and Psychopathology: A Transdiagnostic Deficit with Symptom-Specific Pathways through Emotion Regulation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
115
1
2

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 135 publications
(127 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
9
115
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…One speculation for future research is that this effect represents a more global, valence-general construct of following feelings in affective forecasting (e.g., a general tendency to rely on emotion leading to overprediction of all future affect). Another is that using emotion as information depends on other meta-emotional abilities as inputs for cognitive processing, abilities that are also implicated in psychopathology (e.g., emotional clarity; Vine & Aldao, 2014). The tendency among dysphorics to follow negative feelings may thus capture broader misuse of affective states when forecasting affect (see Hoerger, Chapman, Epstein, & Duberstein, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One speculation for future research is that this effect represents a more global, valence-general construct of following feelings in affective forecasting (e.g., a general tendency to rely on emotion leading to overprediction of all future affect). Another is that using emotion as information depends on other meta-emotional abilities as inputs for cognitive processing, abilities that are also implicated in psychopathology (e.g., emotional clarity; Vine & Aldao, 2014). The tendency among dysphorics to follow negative feelings may thus capture broader misuse of affective states when forecasting affect (see Hoerger, Chapman, Epstein, & Duberstein, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For this purpose, we synthesized and expanded upon previous theories [e.g., 16,17,18,19] and proposed a skill-based model of emotion regulation. According to the Adaptive Coping with Emotions (ACE) Model [20] (Figure 1), effective emotion regulation can be conceptualized as the situation-adapted interplay of the abilities to (a) be aware of emotions [e.g., 21,22], (b) identify and label emotions [e.g., [23,24], (c) correctly interpret emotion-related body sensations [e.g., [25,26], (d) understand the prompts of emotions [e.g., [27], (e) actively modify negative emotions [e.g., [28,29], (f) accept negative emotions when necessary [e.g., 2,19,30,31], (g) tolerate negative emotions when they cannot be changed [e.g., [32,33], (h) confront (vs. avoid) distressing situations in order to attain important goals [e.g., [34,35], and (i) compassionately support (encourage, self-soothe, guide) oneself…”
Section: Theoretical Underpinnings Of Artmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The resulting developmentally normative mismatch between increased emotional volatility combined with an underdeveloped regulation system means that for youths particularly at risk (e.g., children of substance-dependent parents [46], those with environmental or genetic risk [29••], or psychosocial stress [38]), difficulty with emotion regulation is an identifiable, transdiagnostic, early embedded risk for psychopathology in adolescence—including disorders of addiction, as well as internalizing and externalizing problems [3••, 11••, 15, 17, 19, 4749, 50•, 51••, 52, 53••, 5456]. …”
Section: Emotion Regulation Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%