1966
DOI: 10.3758/bf03328043
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Immediate reward followed by extinction vs. later reward without extinction

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

1
25
0

Year Published

1981
1981
2006
2006

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 97 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 6 publications
1
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…and rat, pigeon, and human subjects, preference reversals as a function of temporal remoteness of alternatives have been suggested by the data in a number of studies (Ainslie, 1974;Deluty, 1978;Fantino, 1966;Logan & Spanier, 1970;Navarick & Fantino, 1976;Rachlin & Green, 1972;Solnick, Kannenberg, Eckerman, & Walker, 1980). However, each of these studies has had limitations.…”
Section: Subjectsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…and rat, pigeon, and human subjects, preference reversals as a function of temporal remoteness of alternatives have been suggested by the data in a number of studies (Ainslie, 1974;Deluty, 1978;Fantino, 1966;Logan & Spanier, 1970;Navarick & Fantino, 1976;Rachlin & Green, 1972;Solnick, Kannenberg, Eckerman, & Walker, 1980). However, each of these studies has had limitations.…”
Section: Subjectsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In both experiments, choice responding was more sensitive to variations in reinforcer duration at overall longer reinforcer delays than at overall shorter reinforcer delays, independently of whether fixed-interval or variableinterval schedules were arranged in the choice phase. We concluded that preference reversal results from a change in sensitivity of choice responding to ratios of reinforcer duration as the delays to both reinforcers are lengthened.Key words: self-control, choice, preference reversal, generalized matching law, sensitivity to reinforcer amount, reinforcer delay, concurrent-chains schedules, key peck, pigeonsIn the typical self-control procedure, a concurrent choice is arranged between an immediate short-duration reinforcer and a delayed longer duration reinforcer (Fantino, 1966). We speak of self-control when the delayed longer duration reinforcer is chosen (Ainslie, 1974(Ainslie, , 1975Rachlin, 1974;Rachlin & Green, 1972).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the typical self-control procedure, a concurrent choice is arranged between an immediate short-duration reinforcer and a delayed longer duration reinforcer (Fantino, 1966). We speak of self-control when the delayed longer duration reinforcer is chosen (Ainslie, 1974(Ainslie, , 1975Rachlin, 1974;Rachlin & Green, 1972).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, this model also predicts choice of the smaller, more immediate reinforcer over the larger, more delayed reinforcer as time to the larger reinforcer increases (Fantino, 1966;Ito & Asaki, 1982;Navarick & Fantino, 1976).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A number of behavioral researchers have analyzed self-control from this perspective (Ainslie, 1974;Fantino, 1966;Navarick & Fantino, 1976;Rachlin & Green, 1972), presenting alternatives differing in amount and delay of reinforcement. Studies analyzing the distribution of choice (Ainslie & Herrnstein, 1981; Green, Fisher, Perlow, & Sherman, 1981) have found that an organism's behavior could often be described by the matching law (Herrnstein, 1970), which predicts a match between the proportion of responses and the proportion of reinforcers for that response.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%