2014
DOI: 10.1242/bio.20149183
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Immediate and punitive impact of mechanosensory disturbance on olfactory behaviour of larval Drosophila

Abstract: The ability to respond to and to learn about mechanosensory disturbance is widespread among animals. Using Drosophila larvae, we describe how the frequency of mechanosensory disturbance (‘buzz’) affects three aspects of behaviour: free locomotion, innate olfactory preference, and potency as a punishment. We report that (i) during 2–3 seconds after buzz onset the larvae slowed down and then turned, arguably to escape this situation; this was seen for buzz frequencies of 10, 100, and 1000 Hz, (ii) innate olfacto… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In neither case, however, have systematic analyses of one-trial associative learning been reported. Rather, analyses of associative learning in the larva have so far focused almost exclusively on three-trial conditioning, whether for various sugars (Scherer et al 2003;Schipanski et al 2008;Rohwedder et al 2012) and amino acids as taste rewards (Schleyer et al 2015a), for optogenetically induced reward learning through the activation of subsets of dopaminergic neurons (Saumweber et al 2018), for punishment by substrate vibration (Eschbach et al 2011;Saumweber et al 2014) or electric shocks (Pauls et al 2010), or for quinine as taste punishment (Gerber and Hendel 2006;El-Keredy et al 2012;Apostolopoulou et al 2014). Only for salt, that is high concentrations of sodium chloride, have analyses of three-trial learning (Gerber and Hendel 2006;Niewalda et al 2008) recently been complemented by an experiment reporting associative memory after just one training trial (Widmann et al 2016; loc.…”
Section: [Supplemental Materials Is Available For This Article]mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In neither case, however, have systematic analyses of one-trial associative learning been reported. Rather, analyses of associative learning in the larva have so far focused almost exclusively on three-trial conditioning, whether for various sugars (Scherer et al 2003;Schipanski et al 2008;Rohwedder et al 2012) and amino acids as taste rewards (Schleyer et al 2015a), for optogenetically induced reward learning through the activation of subsets of dopaminergic neurons (Saumweber et al 2018), for punishment by substrate vibration (Eschbach et al 2011;Saumweber et al 2014) or electric shocks (Pauls et al 2010), or for quinine as taste punishment (Gerber and Hendel 2006;El-Keredy et al 2012;Apostolopoulou et al 2014). Only for salt, that is high concentrations of sodium chloride, have analyses of three-trial learning (Gerber and Hendel 2006;Niewalda et al 2008) recently been complemented by an experiment reporting associative memory after just one training trial (Widmann et al 2016; loc.…”
Section: [Supplemental Materials Is Available For This Article]mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To measure the locomotor changes by mechanosensory disturbance, we used the 'buzz' set-up described in Eschbach et al 2011and Saumweber et al (2014), with minor changes to accommodate the small size of L1. Specifically, (i) a higher-resolution camera (Basler ace acA2040-90umNIR, Ahrensburg, Germany), (ii) new, customwritten software, and (iii) agarose-filled Petri dishes of smaller diameter (90 mm) were used.…”
Section: Video Tracking Of the Response To 'Buzz' Mechanosensory Distmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We measured translational run speed as well as the angular sideways speed of L1 and L3, both under baseline conditions and upon presentation of a disturbing mechanosensory 'buzz' stimulus (Eschbach et al, 2011;Saumweber et al, 2014). Under baseline conditions, translational run speed was about 4-fold lower in L1 than in L3, while angular speed was equal in the two stages (Fig.…”
Section: Mechano-nociception and Touchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The adaptive advantage of this modulation might be to focus the worm on the immediate sensory challenges so that it is not distracted by less relevant mechanosensory input while it tries to escape adverse conditions. In a similar type of study with Drosophila larvae, mechanosensory disturbance − in the form of high frequency “buzzes,” perhaps reminiscent of the sound of parasitoid wasps − reduced behavioral responses to attractive odors .…”
Section: Neural Circuit Integration Of Chemosensory and Mechanosensormentioning
confidence: 99%