1975
DOI: 10.1016/0023-9690(75)90037-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Immediate and delayed reinforcers for flavor preferences in rats

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

16
140
1

Year Published

1993
1993
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 202 publications
(157 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
16
140
1
Order By: Relevance
“…brook, 2000;Warwick & Weingarten, 1994). Furthermore, there is some evidence that they may rely on different psychological mechanisms as palatability-and nutrient-based learning appear to affect appetitive and consummatory responding differently (Myers & Hall, 1998) and manipulations of the temporal relationship between CS and US appear to affect palatability-based preferences more than nutrient-based preferences (e.g., Holman, 1975;. Maltodextrin selectively supports nutrientbased preferences as its taste alone is generally ineffective in conditioning a preference when nutrient effects are minimized (Bonacchi, Ackroff, & Sclafani, 2008;Elizalde & Sclafani, 1988).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…brook, 2000;Warwick & Weingarten, 1994). Furthermore, there is some evidence that they may rely on different psychological mechanisms as palatability-and nutrient-based learning appear to affect appetitive and consummatory responding differently (Myers & Hall, 1998) and manipulations of the temporal relationship between CS and US appear to affect palatability-based preferences more than nutrient-based preferences (e.g., Holman, 1975;. Maltodextrin selectively supports nutrientbased preferences as its taste alone is generally ineffective in conditioning a preference when nutrient effects are minimized (Bonacchi, Ackroff, & Sclafani, 2008;Elizalde & Sclafani, 1988).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Holman (1975) showed that rats acquired a preference for drinking a flavor that had been paired with a concentrated solution of saccharin over another flavor paired with a weaker solution of saccharin. Similarly, Sclafani and Ackroff (1994) have shown that rats will learn a preference for a flavor that is paired with glucose or fructose, and Sclafani (1988, 1990) have shown a similar preference for flavors that have been paired with the hydrolyzed starch, polycose.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Non-nutritive, but palatable, substances will also support the development of preferences to novel flavours that they are paired with [32,76]. As such, both saccharin and mineral oil will condition a flavour preference provided these stimuli are presented at the same time (i.e.…”
Section: Role Of Taste and Post-ingestive Signals In Reinforcement Anmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is termed flavour-taste conditioning and is observed with most palatable compounds including nutritive sugars such as glucose. However, if experiences with the novel flavour and with saccharin are separated in time, in a so-called delayed procedure, then no conditioned response develops [32,33].…”
Section: Artificial Sweetenersmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Revusky, 1968). However, the appetitive association of a sensory cue with a nutritional manipulation (conditioning of preference) was seen with a delay of merely half an hour (Holman, 1975) or up to an hour or so at the most (Sclafani & Ackroff, 1994;Simson & Booth, 1973b). Nevertheless, it should be noted that these stimulus-conditioning experiments used incomplete foods in small amounts and/or unphysiological routes and timings (Mather, Nicolaïdis & Booth, 1978).…”
Section: Learning Over Long Delaysmentioning
confidence: 99%