1993
DOI: 10.1080/14640749308401037
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Hypothesis-Testing Strategies: Why two Goals are Better than One

Abstract: An intriguing finding in the hypothesis-testing literature concerns a large increase in the proportion of subjects who discover a rule when they are asked to determine two rules rather than that rule alone. This finding is based on Wason's (1960) “2 4 6” task, in which subjects try to discover a rule (ascending numbers) by generating and testing number triples. They are initially given an example (“2, 4, 6”) of the rule that leads to overly specific hypotheses. With single-goal (SG) instructions, subjects try … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
36
1

Year Published

1995
1995
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
6
36
1
Order By: Relevance
“…We note that the success rates presented in Table 1 for the useful versus the nonuseful contrast class cue conditions of Experiment 1 are, in fact, very similar to those typically reported in the literature for the DG version of the task versus the SG version of the task, respectively (e.g., Gale & Ball, 2006;Wharton et al, 1993). This observation supports the notion that the DG instructions per se may not lead to task success in the DG paradigm, but rather the fact that DG instructions facilitate participants' production of a salient contrast class-which, in turn, promotes successful discovery of the target DAX rule.…”
supporting
confidence: 80%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…We note that the success rates presented in Table 1 for the useful versus the nonuseful contrast class cue conditions of Experiment 1 are, in fact, very similar to those typically reported in the literature for the DG version of the task versus the SG version of the task, respectively (e.g., Gale & Ball, 2006;Wharton et al, 1993). This observation supports the notion that the DG instructions per se may not lead to task success in the DG paradigm, but rather the fact that DG instructions facilitate participants' production of a salient contrast class-which, in turn, promotes successful discovery of the target DAX rule.…”
supporting
confidence: 80%
“…The high number of participants producing overly restricted initial DAX hypotheses (which included 9 of the10 solvers) runs counter to the idea that the 6-4-2 MED triple simply removed the inductive challenge and directly primed the correct DAX solution. This view is also challenged by the observation that solvers only reached an awareness of the correct DAX rule after a mean of 4.90 self-generated triples (SD = 1.05), which is similar to the value that has been reported with the standard DG paradigm (e.g., Wharton et al 1993). We therefore see no evidence to support a simple explanation of the facilitatory effects of the 6-4-2 MED triple as arising from the direct seeding of the correct DAX rule from the outset.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 65%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This is related to the proposition (see, e.g., Evans, 1989, p. 60;Klayman & Ha, 1987) that subjects tend to focus on positive information. One clear finding in the research using Wason's 2-4-6 task is that subjects do not readily generate triples to test alternative hypotheses (Wason, 1960), unless the experimental situation is designed to get them to do so (Gorman, Stafford, & Gorman, 1987;Tweney et aI., 1980;Wharton, Cheng, & Wickens, 1993). It is our experience with the 2-4-6 task that subjects simply never spontaneously state two hypotheses when performing a test with a single triple (Tweney et aI., 1980), which is consistent with the proposition that subjects typically consider data as relevant to only one hypothesis at a time.…”
Section: Relation To Other Laboratory Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%