2014
DOI: 10.3141/2458-12
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Human Factors Issues of Accidents at Passively Controlled Rural Level Crossings

Abstract: Collisions between road vehicles and trains at passively controlled level crossings, where no indication is given to drivers of the presence (or absence) of an approaching train, raise important human factors safety concerns. A database search of Canadian level crossing crash data for the 10-year period between 2003 and 2012 and a targeted review of the scientific research literature were conducted to explore the human factors and related risks involved in crashes of this nature. Accidents at passively control… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
10
0
5

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
0
10
0
5
Order By: Relevance
“…This conclusion is in line with the second thesis by Wigglesworth that was mentioned in the introduction of this article. However, a possible alternative explanation that should not be ruled out is that drivers could have committed a kind of looked-but-failed-to-see error (in absence of an immediate threat) as described by Rudin-Brown, French-St. George and Stuart (2014). It is possible that signs that were fixated upon were only processed in a premature sensory stage but did not reach deeper stages of processing like the identification and the retrieval of schemas and scripts connected to it.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This conclusion is in line with the second thesis by Wigglesworth that was mentioned in the introduction of this article. However, a possible alternative explanation that should not be ruled out is that drivers could have committed a kind of looked-but-failed-to-see error (in absence of an immediate threat) as described by Rudin-Brown, French-St. George and Stuart (2014). It is possible that signs that were fixated upon were only processed in a premature sensory stage but did not reach deeper stages of processing like the identification and the retrieval of schemas and scripts connected to it.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Passive crossings represent 67% of public crossing in operation in Australia (Railway Industry Safety and Standards Board, 2009), 75% in the United States (National Transportation Safety Board., 1998), and 47% in Europe (European Railway Agency, 2012a). Passive crossings are mainly located in rural areas, where train speeds are generally faster (e.g., Laapotti, 2015;Rudin-Brown et al, 2014).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Level crossings are therefore governed by a simple rule: the road user must give way to trains. Almost all collisions are the result of the road user failing to obey this rule ( 10 ). At passive crossings, the road user is therefore expected to (i) detect the presence of the crossing, (ii) detect the presence of a train, and (iii) appropriately decide whether it is safe to proceed through the crossing by estimating the train’s speed and arrival time at the crossing.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Overall, the evidence suggests that of all the driver-related factors, unintentional errors are far more commonplace than deliberate violations ( 11 14 ). The main factors leading to such errors are: insufficient sighting distances, or limited conspicuity of the warning signs and/or approaching train; drivers’ lack of awareness of the required response to passive crossing warning signs; drivers’ inability to correctly assess if sufficient time is available to safely complete the crossing ( 15 ), particularly on account of poor ability to estimate the speed of trains as they approach ( 16 ); inattention as a result of complacency caused by low expectations of encountering a train ( 17 ), or distraction ( 18 ); and the so-called “looked-but-failed-to-see” error ( 10 ), in which drivers fail to identify hazards despite looking at the hazard’s source, largely resulting from limitations in human information processing.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%