1977
DOI: 10.1007/978-1-4899-2209-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Human Emotions

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

54
2,487
3
168

Year Published

1997
1997
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3,373 publications
(2,724 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
54
2,487
3
168
Order By: Relevance
“…The emotion of disgust was chosen because this was thought to underlie the effects previously observed in the explicit familiarity task (Stone & Valentine, 2004, in press-b) and the attention orientation task (Stone & Valentine, in press-a). The emotion of disgust serves to protect against physical or psychological contamination and motivates avoidance of the object of disgust (e.g., Charash & McKay, 2002;Druschel & Sherman, 1999;Izard, 1977;Levenson, 1994;Nabi, 2002;Newhagen, 1998;Rozin, Haidt, & McCauley, 1999).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The emotion of disgust was chosen because this was thought to underlie the effects previously observed in the explicit familiarity task (Stone & Valentine, 2004, in press-b) and the attention orientation task (Stone & Valentine, in press-a). The emotion of disgust serves to protect against physical or psychological contamination and motivates avoidance of the object of disgust (e.g., Charash & McKay, 2002;Druschel & Sherman, 1999;Izard, 1977;Levenson, 1994;Nabi, 2002;Newhagen, 1998;Rozin, Haidt, & McCauley, 1999).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Psychological contamination could occur because of association with an unpleasant individual, and disgust has been specifically related to the avoidance of ideas or persons regarded as morally corrupt (Izard, 1977;Nabi, 2002;Rozin et al, 1999). Thus, disgust was thought to underlie the below chance accuracy of explicit familiarity responses to the faces of famous persons evaluated as evil, and the orientation of attention away from these faces.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, it is worth noting that studies of NA and health typically do not control for PA, leaving the interpretation of that literature in regard to valence ambiguous. Although there is much debate regarding the structure of affect (Ekman, 1992;Izard, 1977;Larsen & Diener, 1992;Russell, 1980;Tomkins, 1963;Watson & Tellegen, 1985), when not conceptualized as basic, separate emotions, it has frequently been conceptualized according to a circumplex structure. In this model, affect exists on two dimensions: one describing positive versus negative valence (e.g., happy vs. sad), and one delineating activation levels (e.g., aroused vs. unaroused) (Russell, 1980).…”
Section: Pamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Though surprise clearly involves an emotional reaction (often accompanied by a startle response), it also seems to serve a strategic, cognitive goal, as it directs attention to explain why the surprising event occurred and to learn for the future (e.g., Macedo, 2010;Maguire, Maguire & Keane, 2011;Ranganath & Rainer, 2003). Originally conceived of as a "basic emotion" (e.g., Darwin, 1872;Ekman & Friesen, 1971;Izard, 1977;Plutchik, 1991;Tomkins, 1962), more recently surprise has been re-appraised as a cognitive state because, unlike most emotions, it can be either positively or negatively valenced (Ortony & Turner, 1990; see also Kahneman & Miller, 1986;Maguire et al, 2011). Indeed, nowadays, cognitive aspects of surprise are routinely mentioned in the affective literature; for instance, Wilson and Gilbert (2008) explicitly discuss explanatory aspects of surprise in affective adaptation.…”
Section: A Cognitive Emotion: Theoretical Perspectives On Surprisementioning
confidence: 99%