2013
DOI: 10.1111/evo.12108
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Human Cooperation Based on Punishment Reputation

Abstract: The threat of punishment usually promotes cooperation. However, punishing itself is costly, rare in nonhuman animals, and humans who punish often finish with low payoffs in economic experiments. The evolution of punishment has therefore been unclear. Recent theoretical developments suggest that punishment has evolved in the context of reputation games. We tested this idea in a simple helping game with observers and with punishment and punishment reputation (experimentally controlling for other possible reputat… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
46
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
3
3

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(46 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
0
46
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Until recently, such costly punishment has been perceived as an evolutionary puzzle because punishers accept costs to harm others while third parties benefit from the increased cooperation levels (Dreber et al, 2008;Fehr & Gächter, 2002;Rankin et al, 2009;Sigmund, 2007;Wu et al, 2009). However, if punishers can build up a punishment reputation that affects later decisions of others, punishment can lead to long-term benefits that compensate or even outweigh the immediate costs of punishment (dos Santos et al, 2011;dos Santos et al, 2013;Hilbe & Sigmund, 2010;Hilbe & Traulsen, 2012). This holds for dyadic interactions, but it is unclear whether punishment reputation facilitates the emergence of cooperation also groups larger than 2.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Until recently, such costly punishment has been perceived as an evolutionary puzzle because punishers accept costs to harm others while third parties benefit from the increased cooperation levels (Dreber et al, 2008;Fehr & Gächter, 2002;Rankin et al, 2009;Sigmund, 2007;Wu et al, 2009). However, if punishers can build up a punishment reputation that affects later decisions of others, punishment can lead to long-term benefits that compensate or even outweigh the immediate costs of punishment (dos Santos et al, 2011;dos Santos et al, 2013;Hilbe & Sigmund, 2010;Hilbe & Traulsen, 2012). This holds for dyadic interactions, but it is unclear whether punishment reputation facilitates the emergence of cooperation also groups larger than 2.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When humans were put into the respective situations and in front of a screen that provided all the information necessary to play these simple strategies, they seemed to behave as predicted: reputation based on generosity or on punishment was used, and the use of reputation increased cooperation frequencies (Wedekind and Milinski, 2000;Milinski et al, 2001;Wedekind and Braithwaite, 2002;dos Santos et al, 2013). However, such experiments cannot reveal the strategies humans use.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Apart from reputation based on generosity, there are other kinds of reputational effects that influence cooperation in humans. A reputation based on punishment, for example, may have played a key role in the evolution of punishment that promotes cooperation within groups (Brandt et al, 2003;Gardner and West, 2004;Hilbe and Sigmund, 2010;dos Santos et al, 2011dos Santos et al, , 2013. Various kinds of reputation may therefore affect partner choice (Fu et al, 2008;Sylwester and Roberts, 2010) and may thereby create a biological market (Barclay, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations