2020
DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2020.1840520
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

How does the number of targets affect visual search performance in visuospatial neglect?

Abstract: Introduction: Impairments in visual search are a common symptom in visuospatial neglect (VSN). The severity of the lateralized attention bias in visual search tasks can vary depending on the number of distractors: the more distractors, the more targets are missed. However, little is known about how the number of targets affect search performance in VSN. The aim of the current study was to examine the effect of the number of targets on hit rate in VSN. Methods: We included 23 stroke patients with right-brain da… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
13
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 92 publications
0
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Some studies compared diagnostic accuracy between CB tests, and as expected the more complex CB tasks (e.g., higher demands, dual tasks, increased number of targets, conjunction tasks) were more sensitive and could detect more cases (chronic/subclinical) than more simple versions (e.g., feature tasks) (Marshall et al, 1997;List et al, 2008;Erez et al, 2009;Van Kessel et al, 2013;Andres et al, 2019;Ten Brink et al, 2020;Villarreal et al, 2020).…”
Section: Computer-based Task Typementioning
confidence: 94%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Some studies compared diagnostic accuracy between CB tests, and as expected the more complex CB tasks (e.g., higher demands, dual tasks, increased number of targets, conjunction tasks) were more sensitive and could detect more cases (chronic/subclinical) than more simple versions (e.g., feature tasks) (Marshall et al, 1997;List et al, 2008;Erez et al, 2009;Van Kessel et al, 2013;Andres et al, 2019;Ten Brink et al, 2020;Villarreal et al, 2020).…”
Section: Computer-based Task Typementioning
confidence: 94%
“…The authors identified 13 studies, that investigated test batteries of CB versions of conventional tasks, including tasks similar to line bisection (Chiba et al, 2010;Jee et al, 2015), cancellation (Rabuffetti et al, 2002(Rabuffetti et al, , 2012, baking tray (Chung et al, 2016) or combinations of different types of tasks (Liang et al, 2007;Ulm et al, 2013;Pallavicini et al, 2015;Vaes et al, 2015;Mizuno et al, 2016;Ten Brink et al, 2016;Quinn et al, 2018;Morando et al, 2019). Our synthesis included 11 studies exploring visual search tasks such as static (Mizuno et al, 2016;Machner et al, 2018;Ten Brink et al, 2020), feature and conjunction (List et al, 2008;Erez et al, 2009), dynamic and dual tasks (Marshall et al, 1997;Deouell et al, 2005;Van Kessel et al, 2013;Andres et al, 2019;Villarreal et al, 2020). We detected five studies that observed different types of tasks such as the widely investigated Posner cueing paradigm (Rengachary et al, 2009), a neglect/extinction task (Vossel et al, 2010), a temporal order judgment (TOJ) task (Stigchel and Nijboer, 2017), a driving simulator task (Spreij et al, 2020) and a manual spatial exploration task (Pierce et al, 2021).…”
Section: Computer-based Task Typementioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Patients with milder forms of the deficit, however, tend not to show omissions in cancellation tasks without distractors. To overcome this problem, task sensitivity has been enhanced by specifically adjusting the stimuli, for example, by increasing the target density and the relative salience of distractors (Rapcsak et al, 1989;Aglioti et al, 1997;Chatterjee et al, 1999;Sarri et al, 2009;Bickerton et al, 2011;Basagni et al, 2017;Ten Brink et al, 2020;Villarreal et al, 2020). To prevent compensation and to identify milder deficits, time limits are sometimes introduced (Priftis et al, 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%