2005
DOI: 10.1121/1.4786613
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

How auditory discontinuities and linguistic experience affect the perception of speech and non-speech in English- and Spanish-speaking listeners

Abstract: The present study was designed to investigate the effects of long-term linguistic experience on the perception of non-speech sounds in English and Spanish speakers. Research using tone-onset-time (TOT) stimuli, a type of non-speech analogue of voice-onset-time (VOT) stimuli, has suggested that there is an underlying auditory basis for the perception of stop consonants based on a threshold for detecting onset asynchronies in the vicinity of +20 ms. For English listeners, stop consonant labeling boundaries are c… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

2
19
0

Year Published

2007
2007
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 6 publications
2
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The results replicate the findings of Miyawaki et al (1975) of no cross-language differences for F3 transitions only, but clarify that cross-language differences can be found for other sounds that do not sound consistently like speech. Our results also fit with Hay (2005), who found significant, but diminished, Spanish-English discrimination differences for a tone-onset analog of voice onset time. We think that their non-speech analog is comparable to our middle range of stimuli in Experiment 2 (i.e., close enough to speech to produce cross-language effects but not close enough to sound like speech), and predict that the cross-language difference would be eliminated for stimuli that were less like speech in terms of spectral-temporal properties (e.g., if the tones were moved outside of the range of F1 and F2), and increased for stimuli that were more like speech.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The results replicate the findings of Miyawaki et al (1975) of no cross-language differences for F3 transitions only, but clarify that cross-language differences can be found for other sounds that do not sound consistently like speech. Our results also fit with Hay (2005), who found significant, but diminished, Spanish-English discrimination differences for a tone-onset analog of voice onset time. We think that their non-speech analog is comparable to our middle range of stimuli in Experiment 2 (i.e., close enough to speech to produce cross-language effects but not close enough to sound like speech), and predict that the cross-language difference would be eliminated for stimuli that were less like speech in terms of spectral-temporal properties (e.g., if the tones were moved outside of the range of F1 and F2), and increased for stimuli that were more like speech.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…That being said, our argument about phonemic categorization rested on the results for a single stimulus series, and it is still possible that non-speech series exist that have cross-language discrimination differences. For example, Hay (2005) found some differences between Spanish and English speakers in their discrimination of a tone-onset non-speech analog of voice onset time (Pisoni, 1977), although with differences that were less clear than obtained with speech.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…There is, however, another possible interpretation of the finding that speech and nonspeech elicit similar perceptual grouping patternsnamely, that experience with the rhythmic structure of English caused the English-speaking listeners to perceive both the speech and the nonspeech stimuli through an "English" linguistic filter. There is some evidence that linguistic experience may affect the identification (Bent, Bradlow, & Wright, 2003) and discrimination (Hay, 2005) of some nonspeech sounds, and it is possible that the effects found in the present experiment resulted from language-specific perceptual-grouping strategies, and not from general auditory processing. In Experiment 2, we addressed this possibility.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 66%
“…5). As suggested by Hay (2005), this difference does not favor English-speaking individuals, from whom greater temporal perceptual acuity is requested.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 76%