2005
DOI: 10.1017/s0952836905007089
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Hoverfly mimicry deceives humans

Abstract: It is believed that the resemblance of many hoverflies to stinging hymenopterans is a case of Batesian mimicry, though there is little experimental evidence that it is effective in protecting them from predators. In this study the effectiveness of hoverfly mimicry was investigated for humans by presenting groups of university students and schoolchildren with a questionnaire which included pictures of stinging hymenopterans, mimetic hoverflies and dipteran controls. More people thought that the mimics would sti… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
15
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
0
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It is well known that humans can be regularly duped by even imperfect mimicry (e.g. see Golding, Ennos, Sullivan, & Edmunds, 2005), while Valkonen and Mappes (2014) proposed that viper mimicry is disadvantageous to smooth snakes when encountering humans, who may kill them after mistaking them for venomous species. Moreover, the primacy of colour in overshadowing reported by Kazemi et al was .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is well known that humans can be regularly duped by even imperfect mimicry (e.g. see Golding, Ennos, Sullivan, & Edmunds, 2005), while Valkonen and Mappes (2014) proposed that viper mimicry is disadvantageous to smooth snakes when encountering humans, who may kill them after mistaking them for venomous species. Moreover, the primacy of colour in overshadowing reported by Kazemi et al was .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, birds may pay more attention to other cues (prey traits) than colour patterns (e.g., qualities of flight motion). Thus, a human observer may mistakenly judge a hoverfly as a mimic of a hymenopteran insect (e.g., Golding et al, 2005) when in fact there is no mimicry in the eye of the relevant beholder -the avian predator (for detailed discussion of other explanations for the maintenance of poor Batesian mimicry see Edmunds, 2000;Ruxton et al, 2004;Grim, 2005).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The "carry-over" we observed emphasizes that differences in early learning experiences (which in the wild can be varied and unpredictable) can have persistent effects on later behavior. Several previous studies have adopted a similar approach of using humans as surrogate predators to judge the accuracy of mimics, and show the utility of using human test subjects in enabling a large sample size (Golding et al 2005b;Penney et al 2012;Sherratt et al 2015). Of course, results from human trials can only be applied to real mimetic systems with a degree of caution, although their results tally well with other studies that have used avian predators (Dittrich et al 1993;Kazemi et al 2014).…”
Section: Page 6 Of 11mentioning
confidence: 62%
“…Past studies that have used humans as predators (Golding et al 2005b;Sherratt et al 2015) have (to varying degrees) used the somewhat unrealistic scenario of the subjects being given time to inspect the images at their leisure, and in addition, Penney et al (2012) presented large, high-resolution images. The present work used a short presentation of a low resolution image in order to best resemble the situation in which a predator must make a quick decision based on an insect viewed from a distance, with any hesitation allowing the prey a chance to escape (Chittka and Osorio 2007;Abbott and Sherratt 2013).…”
Section: Page 6 Of 11mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation