2004
DOI: 10.1002/hipo.10189
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Hippocampal activation during transitive inference in humans

Abstract: Studies in rodents have demonstrated that the integration and flexible expression of memories, necessary for transitive inference, depend on an intact hippocampus. To test this hypothesis in humans, we studied brain activation during the discrimination of a series of overlapping and non-overlapping arbitrary visual stimulus pairs. We report that transitive inference about overlapping pairs is associated with right anterior hippocampal activation, whereas recognition of non-overlapping stimulus pairs is associa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

25
233
3

Year Published

2006
2006
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 245 publications
(261 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
25
233
3
Order By: Relevance
“…The hippocampus was selectively activated when people identified the indirect associations between faces that were paired with the same house as compared with direct faceface associations. In another study, subjects were trained on the task which involves a hierarchical series of judgments (A > B, B > C, C > D, D > E) or a series of non-overlapping judgments (K > L, M > N, O > P, Q > R; Heckers, Zalezak, Weiss, Ditman, & Titone, 2004). The hippocampus was activated when subjects performed transitive judgments as compared to novel judgments between items taken from the non-overlapping pairs.…”
Section: Episodes Are Represented As Sequences Of Eventsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The hippocampus was selectively activated when people identified the indirect associations between faces that were paired with the same house as compared with direct faceface associations. In another study, subjects were trained on the task which involves a hierarchical series of judgments (A > B, B > C, C > D, D > E) or a series of non-overlapping judgments (K > L, M > N, O > P, Q > R; Heckers, Zalezak, Weiss, Ditman, & Titone, 2004). The hippocampus was activated when subjects performed transitive judgments as compared to novel judgments between items taken from the non-overlapping pairs.…”
Section: Episodes Are Represented As Sequences Of Eventsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Inference has been studied with paradigms that allow for the conscious encoding and retrieval of episodes (Bunsey and Eichenbaum, 1996;Dusek and Eichenbaum, 1997;Heckers et al, 2004;Preston et al, 2004;Smith and Squire, 2005;Ellenbogen et al, 2007). Participants encode stimulus pairs that contain overlapping elements (A-B, B-C).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For A-C, A is the correct choice. A correct choice enabled by the flexible integration of A-B and B-C memories is considered true inference (Leo and Greene, 2008), which is thought to depend on the hippocampus (Bunsey and Eichenbaum, 1996;Dusek and Eichenbaum, 1997;Heckers et al, 2004;Preston et al, 2004;Smith and Squire, 2005;Ellenbogen et al, 2007) and hence on episodic (Tulving, 2002) or declarative (Cohen and Eichenbaum, 1993;Reber et al, 1996;Squire and Wixted, 2011) memory. However, a correct choice at test may also result from the learning of simple reward associations (A is always rewarded and C never) that relies on the dopaminergic reward system (Frank et al, 2003(Frank et al, , 2006Libben and Titone, 2008;Moses et al, 2010).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this study, we examine schema-based effects in the context of the transitive inference (TI) paradigm-an experimental scenario which provides a well-controlled setting in which to examine the influence of an associative schema (i.e., prior knowledge of the rank position of items in the hierarchy) on the learning of new information (i.e., relating to the rank position of novel items). To the best of our knowledge, however, the TI paradigm has not been employed with this purpose in mind, though it has been widely used across species to study the mechanisms that support inferential behavior and generalization, and in particular the specific contribution of the hippocampus (Bryant and Trabasso 1971; Chalmers 1977, 1992;Harris and McGonigle 1994;Rapp et al 1996;Dusek and Eichenbaum 1997;Delius and Siemann 1998;Greene et al 2001;Frank et al 2003Frank et al , 2005Heckers et al 2004;Titone et al 2004;Smith and Squire 2005;Moses and Ryan 2006;Moses et al 2010;Zeithamova et al 2012).In this three-phase study involving 30 participants, we used a version of the TI paradigm, which follows the lines of the original paradigm developed by Bryant and Trabasso (1971), and has been shown to ensure the development of robust transitivity performance across a group of participants, underpinned by relational knowledge of the hierarchy (see Materials and Methods) (Cohen and Eichenbaum 1993;Eichenbaum 2004;Smith and Squire 2005;. Our experimental design ( Fig.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%