1978
DOI: 10.3758/bf03197452
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Hierarchical organization as a determinant of sequential learning

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

1981
1981
2010
2010

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…There already was some evidence that people can remember 3 to 5 chunks in a situation in which they are to replicate a chessboard, with pauses between groups of pieces defining a chunk (Gobet & Simon 1998). We hoped to extend the evidence to verbal recall, in which there is strong evidence of chunking processes (e.g., Johnson 1978;Marmurek & Johnson 1978). Tulving and Patkau (1962) already found that presenting material with associations between items resulted in the recall of larger chunks, but not more chunks.…”
Section: Does Not Depend On Using a Dual Task To Examine Working Memorymentioning
confidence: 89%
“…There already was some evidence that people can remember 3 to 5 chunks in a situation in which they are to replicate a chessboard, with pauses between groups of pieces defining a chunk (Gobet & Simon 1998). We hoped to extend the evidence to verbal recall, in which there is strong evidence of chunking processes (e.g., Johnson 1978;Marmurek & Johnson 1978). Tulving and Patkau (1962) already found that presenting material with associations between items resulted in the recall of larger chunks, but not more chunks.…”
Section: Does Not Depend On Using a Dual Task To Examine Working Memorymentioning
confidence: 89%
“…Prior research already showed that associations between words could assist in immediate recall (Cumming, Page, & Norris, 2003;Hulme, Stuart, Brown, & Morin, 2003;Stuart & Hulme, 2000). In order to create chunks that varied in size between conditions, Cowan et al (2004) used a training phase to manipulate the association strength within word pairs (for related research, see Anderson & Matessa, 1997;Bowles & Healy, 2003;Johnson, 1978;Marmurek & Johnson, 1978;Ryan, 1969;Slak, 1970;Wickelgren, 1964Wickelgren, , 1967. Cowan et al presented printed words in five training conditions: no-study and zero-, one-, two-, or four-pairing conditions.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The seven-item limit is a description of empirical evidence and therefore is not open to much debate, although it can vary when factors such as word length and phonological similarity are manipulated (Baddeley, 1986). The importance of chunking and grouping is almost universally accepted and has been explored in depth (e.g., Anderson, Bothell, Lebiere, & Matessa, 1998; Bowles & Healy, 2003; Ericsson, Chase, & Faloon, 1980; Frankish, 1985; Frick, 1989; Gobet et al, 2001; Hitch, Burgess, Towse, & Culpin, 1996; Marmurek & Johnson, 1978; Ng & Maybery, 2002; Ryan, 1969; Slak, 1970; Towse, Hitch, & Skeates, 1999; Wickelgren, 1967). Unlike these topics, though, the constant-capacity hypothesis has rarely been tested (though see Tulving & Patkau, 1962).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%