2016
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000916000544
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Hebrew verbal passives in Later Language Development: the interface of register and verb morphology

Abstract: The current study examined the production of Hebrew verbal passives across adolescence as mediated by linguistic register and verb morphology. Participants aged eight to sixteen years and a group of adults were asked to change written active-voice sentences into corresponding passive-voice forms, divided by verb register (neutral and high), binyan pattern (Qal / Nif'al, Hif'il / Huf'al, and Pi'el / Pu'al), and verb tense (past and future tense). Results showed that Hebrew passive morphology is a very late acqu… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Root-and-pattern structure also serves the inflectional expression of verb temporality, as each binyan conjugation actually consists of a set of unique temporal patterns . Table 1 presents the binyan-specific temporal patterns of the five non-passive binyan conjugations (as verbal passive is a very late acquisition in Hebrew and thus not relevant here; see Ravid & Vered, 2017). For example, lamad, lomed, and li-lmod serve as the respective morphological forms of past tense, present tense, and infinitive forms of Qal; while nilmad and yi-lamed denote the respective past and future tense forms of Nif'al.…”
Section: Temporal Inflectionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Root-and-pattern structure also serves the inflectional expression of verb temporality, as each binyan conjugation actually consists of a set of unique temporal patterns . Table 1 presents the binyan-specific temporal patterns of the five non-passive binyan conjugations (as verbal passive is a very late acquisition in Hebrew and thus not relevant here; see Ravid & Vered, 2017). For example, lamad, lomed, and li-lmod serve as the respective morphological forms of past tense, present tense, and infinitive forms of Qal; while nilmad and yi-lamed denote the respective past and future tense forms of Nif'al.…”
Section: Temporal Inflectionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Root and pattern morphology is a type of nonconcatenative morphology found in Semitic languages such as Arabic and Hebrew. This type of morphological patterning is less common among languages in the world, and appears to be more difficult for children to learn (Macwhinney, 1978;Ravid & Vered, 2017;Slobin, 1973). In a series of experiments, Finley and Newport (2018b) demonstrated that English speaking adult and child learners are able to learn root and pattern, nonconcatenative morphology when there are a large number of vowel patterns, but fewer consonantal roots.…”
Section: Morpheme Segmentationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Marked morphological patterns, which are those that are irregular, semantically complex, and phonologically complex, tend to be less frequent than "unmarked" morphological patterns (Haspelmath, 2006). Semantic complexity, phonological regularity, perceptual salience, morphological complexity, and morphological regularity, all affect the learnability of morphology (Harmon & Kapatsinski, 2017;Kelly, Wigglesworth, Nordlinger, & Blythe, 2014;Ravid & Vered, 2017). Morphological complexity can contribute to frequency, and therefore "override" complexity (Kelly et al, 2014;Xanthos et al, 2011).…”
Section: Morphological Categories and Inflectional Paradigmsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The passive form is created by using a root with one of the passive binyanim. Thus, in the past tense, the active/ passive forms of the example roots above (from Ravid & Vered, 2017, p. 1315 are lamad/ nilmad ('learned/was learned'), hisbir/husbar ('explained/was explained') and tipel/tupal ('took care of/was taken care of'). This morphological system is semi-productive, and some roots may be coerced into nonstandard binyanim for special discourse-pragmatic effects (or erroneously by children; adultlike command of the system is achieved only by adolescence; Ravid & Saban, 2008).…”
Section: Study 1: Hebrewmentioning
confidence: 99%