2019
DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-50280-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Heart Rate and Heart Rate Variability Correlate with Clinical Reasoning Performance and Self-Reported Measures of Cognitive Load

Abstract: Cognitive load is a key mediator of cognitive processing that may impact clinical reasoning performance. The purpose of this study was to gather biologic validity evidence for correlates of different types of self-reported cognitive load, and to explore the association of self-reported cognitive load and physiologic measures with clinical reasoning performance. We hypothesized that increased cognitive load would manifest evidence of elevated sympathetic tone and would be associated with lower clinical reasonin… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

4
44
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 52 publications
(49 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
(49 reference statements)
4
44
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This theoretical distinction is powerful because it allows to separate internal and external dimensions of cognitive load and can guide further research (Paas and van Merriënboer, 1994;Choi et al, 2014). However, mental load and mental effort are typically assessed using subjective self-reports on questionnaires (e.g., Krell, 2017), which assumes that the respondents are aware of their actual amount of cognitive load, which they invested to solve a task (Solhjoo et al, 2019). Furthermore, such subjective measures have been critically discussed due to issues of validity (de Jong, 2010;Kirschner et al, 2011;van Gog and Paas, 2008).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This theoretical distinction is powerful because it allows to separate internal and external dimensions of cognitive load and can guide further research (Paas and van Merriënboer, 1994;Choi et al, 2014). However, mental load and mental effort are typically assessed using subjective self-reports on questionnaires (e.g., Krell, 2017), which assumes that the respondents are aware of their actual amount of cognitive load, which they invested to solve a task (Solhjoo et al, 2019). Furthermore, such subjective measures have been critically discussed due to issues of validity (de Jong, 2010;Kirschner et al, 2011;van Gog and Paas, 2008).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, such subjective measures have been critically discussed due to issues of validity (de Jong, 2010;Kirschner et al, 2011;van Gog and Paas, 2008). Hence, some studies use objective, physiological measures as indicators for cognitive load (e.g., various heart rate or pupillometric measures; Solhjoo et al, 2019;Zheng and Cooke, 2012). However, it is not clear to which extent objective measures converge with subjective measures as indicators for an individual's cognitive load in the corresponding contexts.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…From the heart-rate variability, we expect light not to have any effect and cognitive load difference should be visible (similar to [22]).…”
Section: Hypothesismentioning
confidence: 83%
“…Heart-rate variability (HRV) was deeply studied as a cognitive load indicator [22]. The autonomic nervous system contains the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) and the parasympathetic nervous system (PNS), which give respond to the body on activity and rest situations respectively.…”
Section: Heart-rate Variabilitymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation