2022
DOI: 10.1101/2022.05.04.490654
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Heading direction tracks internally directed selective attention in visual working memory

Abstract: We shift our gaze even when we orient attention internally to visual representations in working memory. Here, we show the bodily orienting response associated with internal selective attention is widespread as it also includes the head. In three virtual reality (VR) experiments, participants remembered two visual items. After a working memory delay, a central colour cue indicated which item needed to be reproduced from memory. After the cue, head movements became biased in the direction of the memorised locati… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
2

Relationship

0
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 73 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Location-specific representations can be reconstructed from scalp EEG activity during WM tasks where location is irrelevant (34) and decoded from population spiking activity in prefrontal, parietal, and visual cortex following a cue that renders location irrelevant (e.g., 10). As noted above, human gaze (e.g., 28) and head position (31) are biased towards the location of a behaviorally prioritized item stored in WM, again in tasks where location is irrelevant. Conversely, changes in gaze position or shifts of covert spatial attention during WM can disrupt or introduce systematic biases in memory for visual features (e.g., color; 3536), and memory guided comparisons for successively presented visual stimuli are impaired when they are rendered in different spatial positions (3739).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 88%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Location-specific representations can be reconstructed from scalp EEG activity during WM tasks where location is irrelevant (34) and decoded from population spiking activity in prefrontal, parietal, and visual cortex following a cue that renders location irrelevant (e.g., 10). As noted above, human gaze (e.g., 28) and head position (31) are biased towards the location of a behaviorally prioritized item stored in WM, again in tasks where location is irrelevant. Conversely, changes in gaze position or shifts of covert spatial attention during WM can disrupt or introduce systematic biases in memory for visual features (e.g., color; 3536), and memory guided comparisons for successively presented visual stimuli are impaired when they are rendered in different spatial positions (3739).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…What is meant by “selection”, however, is ill-defined. As summarized elsewhere (3, 5253), several conceptually (and physiologically) distinct signals have been used to track the selection of WM content following changes in behavioral priority, including gaze position (e.g., 28), head position (e.g., 31), scalp EEG (17, 44), fMRI (43), and multi-unit activity (10). Thus, the mechanisms responsible for guiding the selection of WM content are poorly understood, and future research must focus on identifying mechanisms indexed by different physiological measurements and integrating these findings into a coherent model.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…VR is increasingly being used to study visual short-term memory (e.g., Draschkow et al, 2020Draschkow et al, , 2022Thom et al, 2022) by offering a practical and well-controlled way to conduct experiments which incorporate (some of) the complexities of everyday life and to use its sophisticated tracking and stimulation opportunities. Up to now, most of these studies focus on behavioral and eye tracking measures.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%