2019
DOI: 10.1101/665877
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Heading choices of flyingDrosophilaunder changing angles of polarized light

Abstract: 2 Summary Many navigating insects include the celestial polarization pattern as an additional visual cue to orient their travels. Spontaneous orientation responses of both walking and flying fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster) to linearly polarized light have previously been demonstrated. Using newly designed modular flight arenas consisting entirely of off-theshelf parts and 3D-printed components we present individual flying flies with a slow and continuous rotational change in the incident angle of linear … Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
7
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
1
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Flies, like many other insects, display individual heading preferences, maintaining a specific 'goal heading' for periods of time (SI Fig S3) [28,31,43], a behavior that is thought to aid dispersal and long-range navigation [44][45][46][47][48][49]. However, rather than purely fixating on one goal heading, both walking and flying flies also explore other headings while centering their explorations around the goal heading [28,31,43,[50][51][52], a behavioral pattern that matches our observations in this paradigm. These results led us to compare two alternative hypotheses: one in which flies' actions were linked directly to specific visual patterns, as has been suggested to explain fly behavior in this paradigm (see, for example, [53]), and one in which flies' actions were linked to an internal goal heading.…”
Section: Tethered Flying Flies Change Their Visually-guided Behavior After Thermal Conditioningsupporting
confidence: 73%
“…Flies, like many other insects, display individual heading preferences, maintaining a specific 'goal heading' for periods of time (SI Fig S3) [28,31,43], a behavior that is thought to aid dispersal and long-range navigation [44][45][46][47][48][49]. However, rather than purely fixating on one goal heading, both walking and flying flies also explore other headings while centering their explorations around the goal heading [28,31,43,[50][51][52], a behavioral pattern that matches our observations in this paradigm. These results led us to compare two alternative hypotheses: one in which flies' actions were linked directly to specific visual patterns, as has been suggested to explain fly behavior in this paradigm (see, for example, [53]), and one in which flies' actions were linked to an internal goal heading.…”
Section: Tethered Flying Flies Change Their Visually-guided Behavior After Thermal Conditioningsupporting
confidence: 73%
“…Visual attention is a higher-order behavior that requires functional basic vision. We therefore next tested basic motion vision using an optomotor assay with tethered, flying flies in a virtual flight arena 36,37 . Loss of autophagy in photoreceptors did not significantly affect the ability of flies to follow counter-clockwise and clockwise motion (see Methods; Supplementary Fig.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Particularly when navigating over long distances, skylight cues allow the head direction representation to be tethered to global landmarks such as the sun, and to the polarized light patterns of the sky (Heinze and Reppert, 2011). Indeed, polarized light e-vector information has long been thought to be important for the determination of sky-compass-based head direction in many insects, and that is the case in the fly as well (Hardcastle et al, 2020;Mathejczyk and . CC-BY 4.0 International license available under a (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.…”
Section: What the Cx's Network Motifs Tell Us About Its Navigational Computationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The copyright holder for this preprint this version posted December 9, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.12.08.413955 doi: bioRxiv preprint Wernet, 2019;Warren et al, 2018;Weir and Dickinson, 2012;Wernet et al, 2012), despite their comparatively small dorsal rim area (Fortini and Rubin, 1991;Wada, 1974;Wernet et al, 2003)-a dorsal band of the insect eye that is structurally specialized for the detection of polarized light e-vectors in the sky (Labhart, 1999). Sensory information about the celestial polarization pattern reaches the Drosophila CX via a dedicated pathway ( (Hardcastle et al, 2020;Weir et al, 2016), Figures 6-8); although only 5 ER4m neurons from each hemisphere showing strong tuning to e-vector orientation, this tuning collectively covers a large part of the 180 degree range of possible e-vector orientations (Hardcastle et al, 2020;Weir et al, 2016).…”
Section: What the Cx's Network Motifs Tell Us About Its Navigational Computationsmentioning
confidence: 99%