2016
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01892
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Have We Forgotten Auditory Sensory Memory? Retention Intervals in Studies of Nonverbal Auditory Working Memory

Abstract: Researchers have shown increased interest in mechanisms of working memory for nonverbal sounds such as music and environmental sounds. These studies often have used two-stimulus comparison tasks: two sounds separated by a brief retention interval (often 3–5 s) are compared, and a “same” or “different” judgment is recorded. Researchers seem to have assumed that sensory memory has a negligible impact on performance in auditory two-stimulus comparison tasks. This assumption is examined in detail in this comment. … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
40
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 59 publications
(42 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
(67 reference statements)
1
40
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We suppose that the influence, or weight, of a given These hypotheses must be considered tentative, given the scarcity of empirical evidence directly relating memory constraints to auditory prediction. However, the notion of a short-lived memory buffer is consistent with pre-existing concepts of auditory sensory memory (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968;Nees, 2016;Neisser, 1967), and the continuous-decay phenomenon is consistent with well-established recency effects in statistical learning (Bröker et al, 2018;Harrison, 2011;Mattar et al, 2016;Meyniel et al, 2016;O'Reilly, 2013;Squires et al, 1976;Yu & Cohen, 2008).…”
Section: Modelsupporting
confidence: 75%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…We suppose that the influence, or weight, of a given These hypotheses must be considered tentative, given the scarcity of empirical evidence directly relating memory constraints to auditory prediction. However, the notion of a short-lived memory buffer is consistent with pre-existing concepts of auditory sensory memory (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968;Nees, 2016;Neisser, 1967), and the continuous-decay phenomenon is consistent with well-established recency effects in statistical learning (Bröker et al, 2018;Harrison, 2011;Mattar et al, 2016;Meyniel et al, 2016;O'Reilly, 2013;Squires et al, 1976;Yu & Cohen, 2008).…”
Section: Modelsupporting
confidence: 75%
“…Auditory working memory has a more limited informational capacity, and a temporal capacity that can be extended for long periods through active rehearsal. Auditory long-term memory seems to be effectively unlimited in both temporal and informational capacity (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968;Kumar et al, 2016;Nees, 2016;Neisser, 1967).…”
Section: Experiments 3: Memory Decay Helps To Explain the Dynamics Of mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Finally, in the context of dynamic systems, it is also worth remembering that the role of auditory sensory memory may be uncertain; it could still contribute something despite the use of a mask, as was suggested by Nees (). Cowan, Nugent, Elliott, and Saults () found age differences in the temporal decay of auditory sensory memory derived from lists unattended at the time of their presentation and then post‐cued for recall, but with no indication of whether these age difference can survive a mask.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Here we report the results of two experiments investigating the time course of decay of sensory 112 memory (Cowan, 1984;Cowan et al, 1997;Nees, 2016). Auditory sensory memory enables integration 113 of auditory information and preservation of information over brief periods of time (Schröger, 2007).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%