2008
DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.5.1277
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Hands in the air: Using ungrounded iconic gestures to teach children conservation of quantity.

Abstract: Including gesture in instruction facilitates learning. Why? One possibility is that gesture points out objects in the immediate context and thus helps ground the words learners hear in the world they see. Previous work on gesture's role in instruction has used gestures that either point to or trace paths on objects, thus providing support for this hypothesis. Here we investigate the possibility that gesture helps children learn even when it is not produced in relation to an object but is instead produced "in t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
125
1

Year Published

2010
2010
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 155 publications
(132 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
6
125
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Observing others' movements could support episodic memories as encoding of the words relied on the context or the situation elements. Moreover, numerous studies have shown that observing gestures can have positive effects on learning (e.g., Church, Ayman-Nolley, & Mahootian, 2004;Cook, Duffy, & Fenn, 2013;Ping & Goldin-Meadow, 2008). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Observing others' movements could support episodic memories as encoding of the words relied on the context or the situation elements. Moreover, numerous studies have shown that observing gestures can have positive effects on learning (e.g., Church, Ayman-Nolley, & Mahootian, 2004;Cook, Duffy, & Fenn, 2013;Ping & Goldin-Meadow, 2008). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research conducted by Perry et al (1995) showed that children learned more about the mathematical concept of equivalence when the teachers' instructions were accompanied by gestures than by verbal instructions only. Similarly, Ping and Goldin-Meadow (2008) revealed that children who had received instruction with speech and guiding gestures (indicating object sizes) in the explanation of a Piagetian conservation task outperformed children who had received instruction with speech only. In the learning of psychomotor tasks, such as paper folding (Wong et al ), the positive effects of observing movements have been explained by the existence of a mirror-neuron system (Van Gog et al 2009).…”
Section: Observing Movementsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Importantly, making gestures has been shown to lower cognitive load during math problem solving (Goldin-Meadow, Nusbaum, Kelly, & Wagner, 2001) and to foster learning: When instructed to gesture while explaining math problems, children added new problem-solving strategies to their repertoire and remembered more from a subsequent lesson Gestures while Studying Language Animations 5 from the teacher (Broaders, Cook, Mitchell, & Goldin-Meadow, 2007) and this beneficial effect was retained after four weeks (Cook, Mitchell, & Goldin-Meadow, 2008). Observation of gestures was also found to be effective for children's learning (Ping & Goldin-Meadow, 2008). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%