2018
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000918000144
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Growth in syntactic complexity between four years and adulthood: evidence from a narrative task

Abstract: Studies examining productive syntax have used varying elicitation methods and have tended to focus on either young children or adolescents/adults, so we lack an account of syntactic development throughout middle childhood. We describe here the results of an analysis of clause complexity in narratives produced by 354 speakers aged from four years to adulthood using the Expressive, Receptive, and Recall of Narrative Instrument (ERRNI). We show that the number of clauses per utterance increased steadily through t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
25
0
2

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
4
25
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…These school-age L2 children, with an average of 17 months of L2 exposure in school, used more complex sentences in their language production than monolingual four-year-olds, as reported in Diessel (2004) and Vasilyeva et al (2008): 18% of all complete sentences were complex in the language samples of these L2 children (Paradis et al, 2017, p. 9). In Frizelle et al (2018), English L1 children in the age bands from 5 to 7 years produced complex sentences as approximately 22-30% of all utterances, which is more than the L2 children in Paradis et al (2017). But, considering the gap in length of exposure to English between these bilinguals and monolinguals (less than 2 years vs. 5-7 years), the sequential bilinguals are not lagging far behind.…”
Section: Complex Sentences In Production: Bilinguals With Td and With Dldmentioning
confidence: 91%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…These school-age L2 children, with an average of 17 months of L2 exposure in school, used more complex sentences in their language production than monolingual four-year-olds, as reported in Diessel (2004) and Vasilyeva et al (2008): 18% of all complete sentences were complex in the language samples of these L2 children (Paradis et al, 2017, p. 9). In Frizelle et al (2018), English L1 children in the age bands from 5 to 7 years produced complex sentences as approximately 22-30% of all utterances, which is more than the L2 children in Paradis et al (2017). But, considering the gap in length of exposure to English between these bilinguals and monolinguals (less than 2 years vs. 5-7 years), the sequential bilinguals are not lagging far behind.…”
Section: Complex Sentences In Production: Bilinguals With Td and With Dldmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Use of complex sentences in conversation or narrative production can be measured using clausal density: how many clauses per sentence on average (Frizelle et al, 2018) or by comparing the proportion of simple vs. complex sentences used (Paradis et al, 2017). In either case, this measure of growth in complex syntax is focused on use of multi-clause sentences specifically.…”
Section: Clausal Density Versus Mean Length Of Utterancementioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…For this variable, length of residence accounts for 14% of the variance and there is no effect of education, and no interaction. This is surprising: it is well established that in literate speakers, clausal density increases steadily throughout childhood and adolescence (Frizelle et al 2018;Nippold et al 2005;Scott 1988). This increase is most likely attributable to exposure to written texts (Dąbrowska forthcoming).…”
Section: Regression Analysesmentioning
confidence: 99%