1998
DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.00039
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Grasping the Nature of Pictures

Abstract: Abstract

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

4
166
0
9

Year Published

2002
2002
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 214 publications
(179 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
4
166
0
9
Order By: Relevance
“…Previous research has shown that infants understand that static images are representations of objects, and they can discriminate between real objects and depictions of objects (DeLoache, Pierroutsakos, Uttal, Rosengren, & Gottlieb, 1998). Particularly relevant to this study, prior research shows that infants can follow gaze information in videotaped images.…”
mentioning
confidence: 85%
“…Previous research has shown that infants understand that static images are representations of objects, and they can discriminate between real objects and depictions of objects (DeLoache, Pierroutsakos, Uttal, Rosengren, & Gottlieb, 1998). Particularly relevant to this study, prior research shows that infants can follow gaze information in videotaped images.…”
mentioning
confidence: 85%
“…It also shows that, despite significant 8 delays in learning and transfer of information from picture books, children from a pictorially sparse culture begin to treat pictures referentially, as a source of information about the world, by the end of their 3rd year of life. There have been a variety of proposals regarding the particular types of experiences that may be necessary for the early development of pictorial understanding during infancy, ranging from early exposure to two-dimensional images (e.g., Bovet & Vauclair, 2000;DeLoache et al, 1979;Deregowski, 1989), independent exploration of pictures (e.g., DeLoache, 1991;DeLoache et al, 1998;Fletcher & Sabo, 2006;Murphy, 1978), cultural experiences with other people using pictures and other symbols to communicate information about the world (e.g., Callaghan et al, 2011;Callaghan & Rankin, 2002;DeLoache & Burns, 1994;Gelman et al, 2005), or some combination of these factors. Future cross-cultural research examining the specific skills involved in the development of referential understanding of pictures and the type of experiences that affect these skills could reveal important information about mechanisms underlying symbolic development.…”
Section: Learning and Transfer From Pictures In Tanzaniamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nevertheless, the empirical evidence for the facilitative role of early pictorial experience on infants' understanding of pictures is scarce, because the development of pictorial understanding has been examined almost exclusively with children from picture-rich societies (but see Callaghan et al, 2011;DeLoache, Pierroutsakos, Uttal, Rosengren, & Gottlieb, 1998).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…El uso en la condición igual cantidad de unidades gráficas de tarjetas dibujo con tres signos distintos alineados y próximos, al ser una organización espacial poco común para esta representación, puede dificultar una diferenciación precoz entre motivado y arbitrario. Esta hipótesis se fundamenta también en los conocimientos sobre los dibujos que los niños suelen haber elaborado ya a esta edad: no los tratan como objetos físicos que pueden asimilar a sus esquemas sensorio-motores (que se tocan, frotan o golpean como sus referentes), sino como objetos de contemplación que se miran, señalan y respetan una orientación convencional (DeLoache, Pierroutsakos, Uttal, Rosengren & Gottlieb, 1998;DeLoache, Uttal & Pierroutsakos, 2000;Pierroutsakos & DeLoache, 2003). Además, comienzan a comprender el significado simbólico de la relación dibujo-referente (Ganea, Allen, Butler, Carey & DeLoache, 2009;Geraghty, Waxman & Gelman, 2014;Preissler & Carey, 2004;Vivaldi & Salsa, 2014).…”
unclassified