1960
DOI: 10.2307/1419941
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Geometrical Factors in Illusions of Direction

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0
1

Year Published

1972
1972
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
4
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Turning to the RRA tendency we regard this as orientational contrast which over certain limits will vary in the amount induced in one part of the figure by the other, according to the disparity in the number of signals of the two different orientations given by the two different parts. Thus, lengthening the transversals or increasing their density will increase the Zollner illusion (Maheux et al, 1960;Wallace & Crampin, 1969) whilst decreasing the proximity of the two parts of the figure to each other will decrease it (Wallace & Crampin, 1969) ; making the two parts more equal with regard to line length will cause both to show the contrast effect (Hotopf & Ollerearnshaw, 19723); reversing the relative number of signals the two parts give will change the inducing part into the induced part, i.e. change the Zollner into the Poggendorff; lengthening the transversal in the Poggendorff will reduce the effect (Burmester, 1896); and reducing beyond a certain point the difference in orientation between the two parts will change a contrast into an assimilation effect (Imai, 1962;Wallace & Crampin, 1969;Bouma & Andriessen, 1970;Wallace & Moulden, 1973).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Turning to the RRA tendency we regard this as orientational contrast which over certain limits will vary in the amount induced in one part of the figure by the other, according to the disparity in the number of signals of the two different orientations given by the two different parts. Thus, lengthening the transversals or increasing their density will increase the Zollner illusion (Maheux et al, 1960;Wallace & Crampin, 1969) whilst decreasing the proximity of the two parts of the figure to each other will decrease it (Wallace & Crampin, 1969) ; making the two parts more equal with regard to line length will cause both to show the contrast effect (Hotopf & Ollerearnshaw, 19723); reversing the relative number of signals the two parts give will change the inducing part into the induced part, i.e. change the Zollner into the Poggendorff; lengthening the transversal in the Poggendorff will reduce the effect (Burmester, 1896); and reducing beyond a certain point the difference in orientation between the two parts will change a contrast into an assimilation effect (Imai, 1962;Wallace & Crampin, 1969;Bouma & Andriessen, 1970;Wallace & Moulden, 1973).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, ZoÈ llner illusion becomes greater when the number of inducing obliques is higher (Wallace & Crampin, 1969;Maheux, Townsend, & Gresock, 1960).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Wertykalne dłuższe linie wydają się odchylać od pionu w jedną i drugą stronę, podczas gdy na rysunku są w rzeczywistości równoległe. Złudzenie to można zaobserwować, kiedy kąt ostry między liniami krótszymi a linią dłuższą wynosi od 0 do 90 stopni, jednak największy efekt daje kąt od 10 do 30 stopni (Maheux, Townsend, Gresock, 1960). Iluzja działa silniej, kiedy obraz jest zrotowany o 45 stopni, niż kiedy długie linie są poziome lub pionowe.…”
Section: Charakterystyka Badanych Złudzeń Wzrokowychunclassified