2019
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1817706116
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Generic language in scientific communication

Abstract: Scientific communication poses a challenge: To clearly highlight key conclusions and implications while fully acknowledging the limitations of the evidence. Although these goals are in principle compatible, the goal of conveying complex and variable data may compete with reporting results in a digestible form that fits (increasingly) limited publication formats. As a result, authors’ choices may favor clarity over complexity. For example, generic language (e.g., “Introverts and extraverts require different lea… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

5
89
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 92 publications
(109 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
5
89
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This idea that generic statements may communicate stereotypes about unmentioned groups builds on a growing body of research demonstrating the power of generic statements in transmitting social stereotypes about explicitly mentioned groups to young children—by signaling how to carve up the social world into meaningful categories (Rhodes, Leslie, Bianchi, & Chalik, 2018), promoting essentialist beliefs about social groups (Rhodes et al, 2012), and consequently increasing social stereotyping and negative intergroup relations (e.g., Hammond & Cimpian, 2017; Leslie, 2017; Rhodes, Leslie, Saunders, Dunham, & Cimpian, 2018). Although we primarily focused on children, generic statements also bias the beliefs of adults (as confirmed here, among other studies, e.g., Cimpian et al, 2010; Rhodes et al, 2012) and have broad consequences for reasoning across domains, including how they interpret and value scientific research (DeJesus, Callanan, Solis, & Gelman, 2019).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 57%
“…This idea that generic statements may communicate stereotypes about unmentioned groups builds on a growing body of research demonstrating the power of generic statements in transmitting social stereotypes about explicitly mentioned groups to young children—by signaling how to carve up the social world into meaningful categories (Rhodes, Leslie, Bianchi, & Chalik, 2018), promoting essentialist beliefs about social groups (Rhodes et al, 2012), and consequently increasing social stereotyping and negative intergroup relations (e.g., Hammond & Cimpian, 2017; Leslie, 2017; Rhodes, Leslie, Saunders, Dunham, & Cimpian, 2018). Although we primarily focused on children, generic statements also bias the beliefs of adults (as confirmed here, among other studies, e.g., Cimpian et al, 2010; Rhodes et al, 2012) and have broad consequences for reasoning across domains, including how they interpret and value scientific research (DeJesus, Callanan, Solis, & Gelman, 2019).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 57%
“…Authors of narrative reviews tend to report stronger conclusions in the abstracts than in the discussion sections among the study types. Word limits of abstracts may force authors to generate more generic conclusions [ 59 ]. As most readers may only read the title and abstract, we think the abstract should already convey the right message to begin with.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The majority of psychology publications fail to report the racial demographics of their samples (DeJesus et al, 2019) or report simplified dichotomies (e.g., White vs. non-White). Moving forward, authors could report the breakdown of the full racial demographics of their samples (e.g., 70% White, 20% Asian, 8% Black, 2% Multiracial).…”
Section: Moving Forwardmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet decades of critiques advocating for this seem to have gone unnoticed (see Arnett, 2008; Bell & Hertz, 1976; Betancourt & López, 1993; Dunham & Olson, 2016; Graham, 1992; Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010; Kline, Shansudhenn, & Broesch, 2018; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; McLoyd, 1990; McLoyd & Randolph, 1984; Medin, Ojalehto, Marin, & Bang, 2017; Nielsen & Haun, 2016; Nielsen, Haun, Kärtner, & Legare, 2017; Rowley & Camacho, 2015; Syed, 2017; Zuckerman, 1990). In fact, DeJesus, Callanan, Solis, and Gelman (2019) found that across 1,149 articles published in 2015 and 2016 in 11 psychology journals, 73% of them never even mentioned the race of their participants.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%