2005
DOI: 10.1007/s10162-005-5055-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Generalization of Frequency Discrimination Learning Across Frequencies and Ears: Implications for Underlying Neural Mechanisms in Humans

Abstract: Frequency discrimination thresholds (FDTs) at 750, 1500, 3000, and 6000 Hz were measured in 32 normal-hearing listeners before and after each listener practiced the task for 12 h at one of the above frequencies using a single ear. Marked improvements in thresholds taking place over several hours were observed during the frequency-and ear-specific training period. Comparisons between pre-and posttraining thresholds showed large improvements at the trained frequency, but also at other frequencies. The improvemen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
36
1

Year Published

2008
2008
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
1
36
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In a number of experiments, listeners who were trained at one frequency also improved at untrained frequencies. Such generalization has been observed among 0.75, 1.5, 3 and 6 kHz (nZ8 per group; Delhommeau et al 2005), between 5 (nZ8) and 8 kHz (nZ8) (Irvine et al 2000), from 1 to 1.1 and 2 kHz (nZ5, 350 trials per session; Roth et al 2003), from 1 to 0.5, 2 and 4 kHz (nZ12; Amitay et al 2005), and from 3 to 1.2 and 6.5 kHz (nZ8; Demany & Semal 2002). However, in nearly every case, there was some indication that this generalization was not complete.…”
Section: Generalization Across Frequencymentioning
confidence: 96%
“…In a number of experiments, listeners who were trained at one frequency also improved at untrained frequencies. Such generalization has been observed among 0.75, 1.5, 3 and 6 kHz (nZ8 per group; Delhommeau et al 2005), between 5 (nZ8) and 8 kHz (nZ8) (Irvine et al 2000), from 1 to 1.1 and 2 kHz (nZ5, 350 trials per session; Roth et al 2003), from 1 to 0.5, 2 and 4 kHz (nZ12; Amitay et al 2005), and from 3 to 1.2 and 6.5 kHz (nZ8; Demany & Semal 2002). However, in nearly every case, there was some indication that this generalization was not complete.…”
Section: Generalization Across Frequencymentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Transfer of learning involves the application of skills or knowledge learned in one context to another context ͑Cormier and Hagman, 1987;Haskell, 2001;Thorndike and Woodforth, 1901͒. Transfer of learning has been found in the auditory domain for nonspeech stimuli ͑Delhommeau et al, 2005;Delhommeau et al, 2002͒ andspeech stimuli ͑Bradlow andBent, 2008;McClaskey et al, 1983;Tremblay et al, 1997͒. Transfer of learning was, for instance, reported for auditory frequency discrimination tasks: Delhommeau et al ͑2002͒ measured listeners' frequency discrimination thresholds ͑FDTs͒ ͑the smallest audible difference frequency, ⌬f, around a center frequency͒ for four center frequencies ͑750, 1500 3000, and 6000 Hz͒ before and after training. Listeners were then trained for a specific center frequency ͑e.g., 750 Hz͒ and then subsequently tested again at all four center frequencies.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The practical reason for employing log transformation considerations is that the logarithmic transformation helps to avoid a violation of the homoscedasticity assumption of the parametric statistical tests, which is a result of extensive variability of frequency discrimination thresholds, which increases with their magnitude. It is important to note that the use of a logarithmic or square-root transformation of frequency discrimination thresholds is common practice in the psychoacoustic literature (Delhommeau, Micheyl, & Jouvent, 2005;Demany & Semal, 2002;Irvine, Martin, Klimkeit, & Smith, 2000;Micheyl, Delhommeau, Perrot, & Oxenham, 2006). 3.242, p < .01.…”
Section: Psychoacoustic Threshold Comparisonmentioning
confidence: 99%