2021
DOI: 10.3389/fevo.2021.655086
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Generalization of Foraging Experience Biases Bees Toward Flowers With Complex Morphologies

Abstract: The importance of pollinators as selective agents for many floral traits is well established, but understanding their role in the evolution of complex floral shapes remains challenging. This is because pollinators often need much practice to efficiently handle morphologically complex flowers and extract their food rewards. What induces foragers to persistently visit and pollinate complex flowers despite their initial low profitability? We previously found that naive bumblebees, and unsuccessful feeding attempt… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
7
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
2
2

Relationship

1
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
1
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Furthermore, similarities in motor routines in co-flowering species, alongside the inherent complexity of each motor routine, might also affect bees’ decisions to switch, as bees can transfer motor routines they have learned from one species to another, reducing the cost of learning (Ishii & Kadoya, 2016; Krishna & Keasar, 2021; Richman et al, 2021).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, similarities in motor routines in co-flowering species, alongside the inherent complexity of each motor routine, might also affect bees’ decisions to switch, as bees can transfer motor routines they have learned from one species to another, reducing the cost of learning (Ishii & Kadoya, 2016; Krishna & Keasar, 2021; Richman et al, 2021).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These include serial reversal learning (Mota & Giurfa, 2010;Strang & Sherry, 2014), navigation (Cheng, 2005;Cheng & Wignall, 2006), learning the cue that indicates nectar (Dukas, 1995), nest (Colborn et al, 1999;Fauria et al, 2002;Worden et al, 2005), or oviposition site (Weiss & Papaj, 2003). Much research on transfer and interference has focused on learning how to handle flowers to get nectar rewards (Chittka & Thomson, 1997;Gegear & Laverty, 1998;Goulson et al, 1997;Krishna & Keasar, 2021;Laverty, 1994aLaverty, , 1994bLewis, 1986;Lichtenberg et al, 2020;Raine & Chittka, 2007;Woodward & Laverty, 1992).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Bumble bees use a variety of flowers in the wild and associate traits of those flowers (e.g., colour, scent, pattern or shape) with rewards such as nectar (Clarke et al, 2013;Dyer et al, 2006;Giurfa et al, 1996;Gumbert, 2000;Whitney et al, 2009;Wright & Schiestl, 2009). Accessing nectar from flowers require different motor routines depending on flower morphology (Krishna & Keasar, 2021;Laverty, 1994b).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Bumblebees show a mild but persistent innate preference for complex flowers over simplified ones in flight-room experiments. Early training on complex artificial flowers, especially if paired with high reward, improves their learning of a second complex morphology that requires a different handling technique [ 29 ]. These findings suggest behavioural mechanisms that help overcome the initial learning costs of handling complex flowers.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%