2005
DOI: 10.3758/bf03195338
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Gaze aversion: A response to cognitive or social difficulty?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

15
167
3

Year Published

2011
2011
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 203 publications
(195 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
15
167
3
Order By: Relevance
“…A further possibility is that the "regressions" recorded in the illegible condition often reflected a simple recentering of the gaze: The question sentences ended toward the right edge of the screen, and we might not expect participants to hold their gaze to the right at this point. Participants were also answering a question, so the "regressions" may have reflected a kind of gaze aversion (e.g., Doherty-Sneddon & Phelps, 2005). This recentering account fits with the interactions between legibility and target position on regression depth and on regression error: Untargeted recentering fixations would land farther from early targets, which were nearer the left edge of the screen, than from late targets.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A further possibility is that the "regressions" recorded in the illegible condition often reflected a simple recentering of the gaze: The question sentences ended toward the right edge of the screen, and we might not expect participants to hold their gaze to the right at this point. Participants were also answering a question, so the "regressions" may have reflected a kind of gaze aversion (e.g., Doherty-Sneddon & Phelps, 2005). This recentering account fits with the interactions between legibility and target position on regression depth and on regression error: Untargeted recentering fixations would land farther from early targets, which were nearer the left edge of the screen, than from late targets.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, eye movements to blank space are used strategically to avoid automatically processing information: When answering questions, participants often avoid their questioner's gaze, especially when the questions are difficult (Glenberg, Schroeder, & Robertson, 1998). This apparently prevents cognitive overload that might result from fixating the questioner's face and searching for the answer at the same time (Doherty-Sneddon & Phelps, 2005). When reading, any regression will fixate text, and it may be very difficult for the participant to avoid rereading the fixated word.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Participants were also required to ask the experimenter a set of pre-prepared questions and listen to answers given by the experimenter. It has previously been demonstrated that individuals use gaze aversion when processing cognitively demanding information (Doherty-Sneddon et al, 2002;Doherty-Sneddon & Phelps, 2005;Glenberg et al, 1998). In a social interaction people show little gaze aversion when they are listening to another person speak, rather gaze aversion predominantly occurs when people are thinking about a response and when they are speaking (Doherty-Sneddon et al, 2002;Glenberg et al, 1998).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Psycholinguistic research has shown that speakers often avert their gaze when asked questions and that this in turn facilitates remembering and speech planning (Glenberg et al, 1998;Doherty-Sneddon and Phelps, 2005;Markson and Paterson, 2009). Thusthewithdrawalofgazeinthe transition space before a relevant next turn may be a public exponent of a private search for recognition, as is arguably the case in the next extract.…”
Section: Searching For Late Recognitionmentioning
confidence: 99%