2003
DOI: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00372-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Frontal recruitment during response inhibition in older adults replicated with fMRI

Abstract: Recent research has explored age-related differences in multiple areas of cognitive functioning using fMRI, PET, and SPECT. However, because these studies used different tasks, subjects, and methods, little is known about whether the results of these studies are generalizable or repeatable. The present study replicated a previous study [Psychol. Aging 17 (2002) 56] NOT THE PUBLISHED VERSION; this is the author's final, peer-reviewed manuscript. The published version may be accessed by following the link in … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

24
75
4

Year Published

2007
2007
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 100 publications
(103 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
24
75
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Neural compensation refers to the process of recruiting alternative brain networks and structures that are not normally engaged in a certain function. For example, older adults who recruit bilateral (rather than unilateral) prefrontal areas perform better in cognitive tasks than older adults who do not recruit additional brain regions (Cabeza et al 2002;Langenecker and Nielson 2003;Nielson et al 2002;Piefke et al 2012). Such compensatory processes may be enlisted when primary neural networks are no longer able to adequately support successful task performance (ReuterLorenz 2002; Steffener et al 2009), although performance may not be fully maintained.…”
Section: Ageing Lifestyle and Cognitive Reservementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Neural compensation refers to the process of recruiting alternative brain networks and structures that are not normally engaged in a certain function. For example, older adults who recruit bilateral (rather than unilateral) prefrontal areas perform better in cognitive tasks than older adults who do not recruit additional brain regions (Cabeza et al 2002;Langenecker and Nielson 2003;Nielson et al 2002;Piefke et al 2012). Such compensatory processes may be enlisted when primary neural networks are no longer able to adequately support successful task performance (ReuterLorenz 2002; Steffener et al 2009), although performance may not be fully maintained.…”
Section: Ageing Lifestyle and Cognitive Reservementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although some studies report underrecruitment of the neural regions which are activated in young adults (Logan, Sanders, Snyder, Morris, & Buckner, 2002), others find that older adults recruit new regions which are not typically activated in the young (e.g., Morcom, Good, Frackowiak, & Rugg, 2003;Cabeza, Anderson, Locantore, & McIntosh, 2002). Importantly, differences in functional response are also found when behavioral performance is equivalent between age groups (Morcom, Li, & Rugg, 2007;Langenecker & Nielson, 2003) or when age-related performance differences are methodologically controlled (Morcom et al, 2003), demonstrating that fMRI studies may reveal differences in the nature of older adultsʼ responses, which are not evident from behavioral measures alone.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The first hypothesis was that MDD patients would perform less efficiently than the control group on behavioral measures, consistent with our previous findings. The second hypothesis was that MDD patients would exhibit increased activation compared with the control group in areas known to be important for inhibitory control, for example, right inferior frontal, anterior insula, and inferior parietal areas as well as bilateral dorsal AC (Langenecker and Nielson 2003) at comparable levels of performance. It was also expected that the MDD patients would exhibit greater activation for errors of commission compared with the control group in AC and prefrontal areas.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%