2023
DOI: 10.1101/2023.04.22.537940
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Frontal-midline theta and posterior alpha oscillations index early processing of spatial representations during active navigation

Abstract: Previous research has demonstrated that humans combine multiple sources of spatial information, such as allothetic and idiothetic cues, while navigating through an environment. However, it is unclear whether this involves comparing multiple representations from multiple sources during encoding (parallel hypothesis) or primarily accumulating idiothetic information until the end of the navigation to integrate with allothetic information (serial hypothesis). We tested these two hypotheses in an active navigation … Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
1
1

Relationship

0
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Theta power changes during memory encoding often localize to the medial temporal lobes (among other structures), including the hippocampus and parahippocampal regions (Backus et al, 2016;Crespo-García et al, 2016;Fellner et al, 2016Fellner et al, , 2019Griffiths et al, 2016;Hanslmayr et al, 2011;Staudigl & Hanslmayr, 2013). Note, this theta effect is likely distinct from frontal mid-line theta, found during cognitive control and working memory tasks, which typically localizes to medial prefrontal regions (Chrastil et al, 2022;Du et al, 2023;Summerfield & Mangels, 2005;Zuure et al, 2020). Thus, a large body of evidence exists for the role of theta in forming associations between information in both spatial and non-spatial contexts.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Theta power changes during memory encoding often localize to the medial temporal lobes (among other structures), including the hippocampus and parahippocampal regions (Backus et al, 2016;Crespo-García et al, 2016;Fellner et al, 2016Fellner et al, , 2019Griffiths et al, 2016;Hanslmayr et al, 2011;Staudigl & Hanslmayr, 2013). Note, this theta effect is likely distinct from frontal mid-line theta, found during cognitive control and working memory tasks, which typically localizes to medial prefrontal regions (Chrastil et al, 2022;Du et al, 2023;Summerfield & Mangels, 2005;Zuure et al, 2020). Thus, a large body of evidence exists for the role of theta in forming associations between information in both spatial and non-spatial contexts.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, changes in theta oscillatory power have been observed on the scalp during learning of specific items (words/pictures/scenes; Fellner et al, 2013Fellner et al, , 2019Guderian et al, 2009;Hanslmayr et al, 2009Hanslmayr et al, , 2011Khader et al, 2010;Osipova et al, 2006), lists of items (Fellner et al, 2016;Griffiths et al, 2016;Meeuwissen et al, 2011;Sederberg et al, 2006), and during associative learning (Backus et al, 2016;Caplan & Glaholt, 2007;Crespo-García et al, 2016;Griffiths et al, 2016;Joensen et al, 2023;Staudigl & Hanslmayr, 2013;Summerfield & Mangels, 2005). Theta power changes have also been found when participants learn spatial layouts as they combine sensory input in new environments compared to when they freely explore a new spatial layout in the absence of an overt task (Chrastil et al, 2022;Du et al, 2023;Kaplan et al, 2012;Pu et al, 2017). Therefore, a large body of evidence indicates that theta power changes observed on the scalp are important for both nonspatial (episodic) and spatial memory formation.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Theta power changes during memory encoding often localise to the medial temporal lobes (among other structures), including the hippocampus and parahippocampal regions (Backus et al, 2016;Crespo-García et al, 2016;Fellner et al, 2016Fellner et al, , 2019Griffiths et al, 2016;Hanslmayr et al, 2011;Staudigl & Hanslmayr, 2013). Note, this theta effect is likely distinct from frontal mid-line theta, found during cognitive control and working memory tasks, which typically localises to medial prefrontal regions (Chrastil et al, 2022;Du et al, 2023;Summerfield & Mangels, 2005;Zuure et al, 2020). Thus, a large body of evidence exists for the role of theta in forming associations between information in both spatial and non-spatial contexts.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, changes in theta oscillatory power have been observed on the scalp during learning of specific items (words/pictures/scenes; Fellner et al, 2013Fellner et al, , 2019Guderian et al, 2009;Hanslmayr et al, 2009Hanslmayr et al, , 2011Khader et al, 2010;Osipova et al, 2006), lists of items (Fellner et al, 2016;Griffiths et al, 2016;Meeuwissen et al, 2011;Sederberg et al, 2006), and during associative learning (Backus et al, 2016;Caplan & Glaholt, 2007;Crespo-García et al, 2016;Griffiths et al, 2016;Joensen et al, 2023;Staudigl & Hanslmayr, 2013;Summerfield & Mangels, 2005). Theta power changes have also been found when participants learn spatial layouts as they combine sensory input in new environments compared to when they freely explore a new spatial layout in the absence of an overt task (Chrastil et al, 2022;Du et al, 2023;Kaplan et al, 2012;Pu et al, 2017). Therefore, a large body of evidence indicates that theta power changes observed on the scalp are important for both non-spatial (episodic) and spatial memory formation.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%