2008
DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2265-08.2008
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

From Fear to Safety and Back: Reversal of Fear in the Human Brain

Abstract: Fear learning is a rapid and persistent process that promotes defense against threats and reduces the need to relearn about danger. However, it is also important to flexibly readjust fear behavior when circumstances change. Indeed, a failure to adjust to changing conditions may contribute to anxiety disorders. A central, yet neglected aspect of fear modulation is the ability to flexibly shift fear responses from one stimulus to another if a once-threatening stimulus becomes safe or a once-safe stimulus becomes… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

52
398
2
1

Year Published

2010
2010
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 391 publications
(455 citation statements)
references
References 67 publications
(76 reference statements)
52
398
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Similarly, another imaging study showed that conditioned subjects, who had learned to associate one cue with a mild shock and a second cue (safety) with no shock, showed higher amygdala activation during the presentation of the aversive cue, whereas greater striatal activation was found in the presence of the safety cue. When the reinforcement contingencies were reversed, the neural activity pattern in response to the previous fear cue shifted from the amygdala to areas of the ventral prefrontal cortices and the striatum (Schiller et al, 2008). These results are consistent with specific and distinctive neural circuitries subserving learned safety, which involve at least the LA, the striatum, and regions of the PFC (Figure 4).…”
Section: (B) and (D)supporting
confidence: 73%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Similarly, another imaging study showed that conditioned subjects, who had learned to associate one cue with a mild shock and a second cue (safety) with no shock, showed higher amygdala activation during the presentation of the aversive cue, whereas greater striatal activation was found in the presence of the safety cue. When the reinforcement contingencies were reversed, the neural activity pattern in response to the previous fear cue shifted from the amygdala to areas of the ventral prefrontal cortices and the striatum (Schiller et al, 2008). These results are consistent with specific and distinctive neural circuitries subserving learned safety, which involve at least the LA, the striatum, and regions of the PFC (Figure 4).…”
Section: (B) and (D)supporting
confidence: 73%
“…As is the case for experimental research with animal models, learned safety is only beginning to be explored in humans (Grillon and Ameli, 2001;Grillon et al, 1994b;Jovanovic et al, 2010Jovanovic et al, , 2012bLissek et al, 2009;Pollak et al, 2010b;Schiller et al, 2008). Already in the 1990s, pioneering work by Christian Grillon and Michael Davis firstly described the translational potential of learned safety by examining the impact of safety signals on human anxiety and found that safety signals were able to reduce anticipatory anxiety as revealed by a FPS paradigm (Grillon et al, 1994b).…”
Section: Translational Aspects and Potential Applications In Clinicalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the extension of our findings and findings like these to the clinic is premature. Although some studies in humans suggest that immediate behavioral intervention (e.g., extinction) may dampen fear and drug-seeking behavior (Schiller et al 2008;Xue et al 2012), there are human studies that indicate that this approach may have little effect or even enhance memory expression (Karpicke and Roediger 2008;Soeter and Kindt 2011;Wichert et al 2011;Potts and Shanks 2012;Kindt and Soeter 2013). Particularly problematic for clinical applications are rodent and preclinical studies like ours suggesting that across a variety of conditions an immediate extinction experience may actually prevent the elimination or weakening of the fear response and promote its persistent expression (Morris et al 2005;Chan et al 2010;Kim et al 2010).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 78%
“…Several recent studies of memory have asked whether extinction under certain conditions can erase, rather than just suppress, previously established memories (Myers et al 2006;Norrholm et al 2008;Schiller et al 2008;Monfils et al 2009). One popular idea is that extinction during periods of memory lability, which is thought to occur soon after acquisition or retrieval ("immediate extinction"), may permanently displace the original fear memory with a new, safe memory.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Studies with fear conditioning to a discrete cue identified several factors that affect the resistance of extinction to recovery. These include the interval between conditioning and extinction (Myers et al 2006;Woods and Bouton 2008;Huff et al 2009; but see Alvarez et al 2007;Schiller et al 2008;Archbold et al 2010), the interval between extinction and test (Bouton 1993(Bouton , 2004Quirk 2002), the level of fear behavior at extinction (Maren and Chang 2006), and the number and pattern of nonreinforced trails (Cain et al 2003;Urcelay et al 2009). These factors would be expected to interact with each other such that the precise experimental conditions used by independent laboratories would differentially reveal recovery of the cued fear responses.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%