2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.087
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

From a concept to a word in a syntactically complete sentence: An fMRI study on spontaneous language production in an overt picture description task

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
44
0
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 51 publications
(48 citation statements)
references
References 79 publications
3
44
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Less is known about the neural basis of grammatical production in cognitively healthy speakers, but again neuroimaging studies have highlighted the left fronto-temporal-parietal language network for sentence production, in particular inferior frontal regions (Grande et al, 2012; Indefrey, Hellwig, Herzog, Seitz, & Hagoort, 2004; Segaert, Menenti, Weber, Petersson, & Hagoort, 2012). …”
Section: Neural Mechanisms Of Grammatical Processing Deficitsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Less is known about the neural basis of grammatical production in cognitively healthy speakers, but again neuroimaging studies have highlighted the left fronto-temporal-parietal language network for sentence production, in particular inferior frontal regions (Grande et al, 2012; Indefrey, Hellwig, Herzog, Seitz, & Hagoort, 2004; Segaert, Menenti, Weber, Petersson, & Hagoort, 2012). …”
Section: Neural Mechanisms Of Grammatical Processing Deficitsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the domain of language, cognitive control has been shown to play an important role in language production , based on behavioral evidence (e.g., Alm and Nilsson, 2001; Roelofs and Piai, 2011; Strijkers et al, 2011), brain imaging studies (e.g., Müller et al, 1997; Ojemann et al, 1998; Indefrey and Levelt, 2004; Kerns et al, 2004; Haller et al, 2005; Shuster and Lemieux, 2005; Alario et al, 2006; Bohland and Guenther, 2006; Shapiro et al, 2006; Basho et al, 2007; Harrington et al, 2007; Troiani et al, 2008; den Ouden et al, 2009; Eickhoff et al, 2009; Wilson et al, 2009; Brendel et al, 2010; Tremblay and Small, 2011; Adank, 2012; Geranmayeh et al, 2012; Grande et al, 2012; Heim et al, 2012; Delnooz et al, 2013) and investigations of patients with brain damage (e.g., Ziegler et al, 1997; Nestor et al, 2003; Ash et al, 2010; Wilson et al, 2010; Baldo et al, 2011; Coelho et al, 2012; Endo et al, 2013). Indeed, planning and producing linguistic utterances bears intuitive similarity to non-linguistic goal-directed behaviors like reaching (e.g., Bernstein, 1996; Culham and Valyear, 2006; Grafton and Hamilton, 2007; Ridderinkhof et al, 2011) or playing a musical instrument (e.g., Meister et al, 2004).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, demonstrated that the left MTG showed greater activation while processing event verbs (e.g., running) and event nouns (e.g., concert) than while processing object nouns (e.g., strawberry). The role of the left MTG is also considered to involve semantic control because this area was activated in semantic tasks that required efficient semantic retrieval (Gennari et al, 2007;Grande et al, 2012). found that the left MTG was more strongly associated with successful lexical retrieval during continuous spontaneous language production compared with during unsuccessful retrieval.…”
Section: The Left Middle Temporal Gyrus and Oral Proficiency In L2mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We predicted that L2 communication would be modulated by the neural bases of social interaction, eliciting differential engagement of (a) the sensorimotor area associated with action simulation and (b) the deliberative semantic processing area associated with lexical selection as well as the theory-of-mind areas necessary for conceptual generation in both L1 and L2 communication, allowing subjects to compensate for limited communicative skills. The left angular gyrus (AG) and middle and posterior temporal areas, which are associated with lexical storage and semantic concepts (Binder et al, 2009;Braun et al, 2001;Grande et al, 2012), may be sensitive to oral proficiency levels during L2 communication. Furthermore, the neural correlates of L2 communication, but not of L2 description or L1 communication, would be sensitive to the language-related anxiety of individuals.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%