2000
DOI: 10.3758/bf03200831
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Frequency of occurrence and rankings for touch-related adjectives

Abstract: A three-phased study was conducted in order to develop a standardized list of touch-related adjectives. The final list consisted of 306 words that were categorized in 440 instances according to the Lederman and Klatzky (1987, 1990)dimensions of haptic properties (some words were classified in more than one dimension). The Kucera and Francis (1967)frequency of occurrence in written English for all words in the final list was also determined. A correlation was found between frequency of occurrence on the list an… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Texture is particularly salient to touch (Klatzky, Lederman & Reed, 1987), which tends to dominate vision in texture perception (Guest and Spence, 2003) and exceeds vision at discriminating fine textures (Heller, 1989). In addition, the texture words used were predominantly related to touch (see, for example, Stadtlander & Murdoch, 2000; Lynott & Connell, 2009) and many metaphors were likely more easily interpreted by reference to touch than vision. For example, the phrases ‘a rough day’ or ‘a slimy person’ have negative connotations because they refer to attributes that may be particularly unpleasant to touch.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Texture is particularly salient to touch (Klatzky, Lederman & Reed, 1987), which tends to dominate vision in texture perception (Guest and Spence, 2003) and exceeds vision at discriminating fine textures (Heller, 1989). In addition, the texture words used were predominantly related to touch (see, for example, Stadtlander & Murdoch, 2000; Lynott & Connell, 2009) and many metaphors were likely more easily interpreted by reference to touch than vision. For example, the phrases ‘a rough day’ or ‘a slimy person’ have negative connotations because they refer to attributes that may be particularly unpleasant to touch.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Once the message to be communicated by touch has been defined, the challenge becomes one of choosing those tactile sensations that are best suited to deliver that message (e.g., Smets & Overbeeke, 1995). Unfortunately, this step is further complicated by the fact that a proper “lexicon of touch” is still lacking (e.g., Philippe et al, 2003; Spence & Gallace, 2008; though see also Johnson, 2007; Stadtlander & Murdoch, 2000). That is, tactile sensations are not as easily labeled as visual sensations (see Sonneveldt & Schifferstein, 2008).…”
Section: Directions For Future Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For humans, the texture of objects and materials is an important characteristic that determines how they are used and evaluated 9 , 10 . Not surprisingly then, languages generally have dedicated subsets of their vocabulary for the description of texture 11 , 12 , potentially consisting of several hundred words as in the case of English 13 , e.g., rough, smooth, hard, soft, coarse, cottony, fuzzy, silky, oily , and more. Here, we focus on the textural dimension of roughness, which persistently emerges as the dominant perceptual dimension of surface touch when different textures are compared to each other 14 , 15 .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%