1991
DOI: 10.1121/1.401927
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Free-field release from masking

Abstract: Free-field release from masking was studied as a function of the spatial separation of a signal and masker in a two-interval, forced-choice (2IFC) adaptive paradigm. The signal was a 250-ms train of clicks (100/s) generated by filtering 50-/•s pulses with a TDH-49 speaker (0.9 to 9.0 kHz). The masker was continuous broadband (0.7 to 11 kHz) white noise presented at a level of 44 dBA measured at the position of the subject's head. In experiment I, masked and absolute thresholds were measured for 35 signal sourc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

11
61
1

Year Published

1993
1993
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 88 publications
(75 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
11
61
1
Order By: Relevance
“…All of the above mentioned researches discuss the masking release due to directional separation in the horizontal plane. On the other hand, Saberi et al [24] discussed one in the vertical median plane. Because there are no interaural differences for sound presented in the vertical median plane, it is thought to be less advantageous in release from masking due to spatial separation.…”
Section: Release From Masking Due To Separation Of the Sound Sourcesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…All of the above mentioned researches discuss the masking release due to directional separation in the horizontal plane. On the other hand, Saberi et al [24] discussed one in the vertical median plane. Because there are no interaural differences for sound presented in the vertical median plane, it is thought to be less advantageous in release from masking due to spatial separation.…”
Section: Release From Masking Due To Separation Of the Sound Sourcesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…of Directional Hearing Aids The binaural advantage for speech recognition in reverberant environments and a background of noise is well documented for both listeners with and without hearing loss (Arsenault and Punch, 1999;Bronkhorst and Plomp, 1989;Bronkhorst and Plomp, 1992;Byrne, 1981;Carhart, 1965;Moncur and Dirks, 1967;Peissig and Kollmeier, 1997;Saberi et al, 1991;Yost, 1997). Three studies have examined the binaural advantage present for listeners fit with directional and omnidirectional hearing aids (eg, Hawkins and Yacullo, 1984;Nabelek and Mason, 1981;Ricketts, 2000c).…”
Section: Monaural Versus Binaural Fittingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Both NDN and DN noises have been shown to affect target signal detection, recognition, and speech intelligibility (e.g., Abouchacra et al, 1998a;1998b;Dirks and Wilson, 1969;Good et al, 1997;Hirsh, 1950;Lorenzi et al, 1999;Saberi et al, 1991;Thompson and Webster, 1964). Specifically, previous findings suggest that as differences between the target signal and noise become less distinct, the target signal is less perceptible and its location more uncertain.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, many studies have examined the effect of DN (e.g., speech spectrum, white, pink, narrow band noise) on speech detection, recognition, and intelligibility thresholds, and on detection thresholds of nonspeech signals (e.g., Abouchacra et al, 1998;Bronkhorst, 1990;Carhart et al,1969;Dirks and Wilson, 1969;Good et al, 1997;Hirsh, 1950;Kock, 1950;Saberi et al, 1991). Regardless of signal and noise type, results indicate that thresholds of target signals in noise improve as the separation between the target and DN source increases.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%