2017
DOI: 10.1037/rev0000061
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Formalizing Neurath’s ship: Approximate algorithms for online causal learning.

Abstract: Higher-level cognition depends on the ability to learn models of the world. We can characterize this at the computational level as a structure-learning problem with the goal of best identifying the prevailing causal relationships among a set of relata. However, the computational cost of performing exact Bayesian inference over causal models grows rapidly as the number of relata increases. This implies that the cognitive processes underlying causal learning must be substantially approximate. A powerful class of… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

11
157
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 135 publications
(188 citation statements)
references
References 120 publications
11
157
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This finding mirrors research on many other hypothesis-testing tasks suggesting that people have a bias to verify or confirm their hypotheses, even if doing so does not lead to additional information (e.g., Klayman & Ha, 1987;Nickerson, 1998;Ruggeri et al, 2016). In causal intervention tasks, such a tendency has specifically manifested itself in a preference for producing the positive effects (variables turning on) that a particular hypothesis entails (Coenen et al, 2015;Bramley et al, 2017). Because we did not find that participants in the non-sparse group tried to create the expected non-effect, this experiment provides further evidence for this preference to verify positive outcomes in causal systems.…”
Section: Causal Experimentation 22supporting
confidence: 60%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This finding mirrors research on many other hypothesis-testing tasks suggesting that people have a bias to verify or confirm their hypotheses, even if doing so does not lead to additional information (e.g., Klayman & Ha, 1987;Nickerson, 1998;Ruggeri et al, 2016). In causal intervention tasks, such a tendency has specifically manifested itself in a preference for producing the positive effects (variables turning on) that a particular hypothesis entails (Coenen et al, 2015;Bramley et al, 2017). Because we did not find that participants in the non-sparse group tried to create the expected non-effect, this experiment provides further evidence for this preference to verify positive outcomes in causal systems.…”
Section: Causal Experimentation 22supporting
confidence: 60%
“…Evidence for this kind of strategy has been found in different kinds of hypothesis testing experiments in which participants frequently test instances that can confirm their current hypothesis instead of trying to falsify it (e.g., Klayman & Ha, 1987;Wason, 1960;Mckenzie & Mikkelsen, 2000). Recently, a similar tendency was found in a number of causal learning experiments showing that participants were particularly likely to intervene on variables that would yield many effects predicted by a structure that the learner currently considers to be plausible (Bramley et al, 2017;Coenen et al, 2015). There also exists evidence that people in some cases perceive the presence of causal effects as more informative (Coenen & Gureckis, 2015) and more likely (Davis & Rehder, 2017) than non-effects that objectively have the same informational value or likelihood.…”
Section: Stopping Decisionsmentioning
confidence: 68%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The blackbox variational inference algorithm that we use (see Appendix) does in fact involve sampling: the gradient of the evidence lower bound is approximated using a set of samples from the variational approximation. Although we are not aware of direct evidence for such an algorithm in brain or behavior, the idea that hypothesis sampling is involved in the learning process is an intriguing possibility that has begun to be studied more systematically (N. Bramley, Rothe, Tenenbaum, Xu, & Gureckis, 2018;N. R. Bramley, Dayan, Griffiths, & Lagnado, 2017;Rule, Schulz, Piantadosi, & Tenenbaum, 2018).…”
Section: Connections To Hypothesis Samplingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, it has recently been suggested that, at least when considering more global hypotheses about the world where the hypothesis space becomes particularly complex, only one hypothesis would plausibly be represented (Bramley, Dayan, Griffiths, & Lagnado, 2017). In other words, one could presumably replace a larger number of relatively "dumb" particles/hypotheses with a smaller number of comparatively "smart" particles/hypotheses.…”
Section: Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%