2019
DOI: 10.1177/0956797619859531
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Forgetting Is a Feature, Not a Bug: Intentionally Forgetting Some Things Helps Us Remember Others by Freeing Up Working Memory Resources

Abstract: In the present study, we used an item-method directed-forgetting paradigm to test whether instructions to forget or remember one item affect memory for subsequently studied items. In two experiments ( Ns = 138 and 33, respectively), recall was higher when a word pair was preceded during study by a to-be-forgotten word pair. This effect was cumulative: Performance increased when more preceding study items were to be forgotten. The effect decreased when memory was conditioned on instructions for items appearing … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
21
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
(113 reference statements)
1
21
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The first 10 experiments used the same pool of items—180 high-frequency English words for concrete objects from the stimulus pool used by Popov et al (2019). All words were five to seven letters in length.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The first 10 experiments used the same pool of items—180 high-frequency English words for concrete objects from the stimulus pool used by Popov et al (2019). All words were five to seven letters in length.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…By contrast, in the literature on directed forgetting in episodic LTM, testing the to-be-forgotten items by a surprise test is common and forms the very basis for assessing the success of intentional forgetting (e.g., Bjork, 1970; Fawcett & Taylor, 2008; MacLeod, 1999; Popov et al, 2019). During a learning phase of such paradigms, either a list of items or each individual item is followed by a remember or forget instruction.…”
Section: Indirect Assessment Of Memory For To-be-forgotten Informationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Cognitive resources are defined as a limited quantity enabling cognitive functions and processes (Oberauer et al, 2016;Shenhav et al, 2017). In this view, memory is a resourcelimited process (Anderson et al, 1996;Barrouillet et al, 2004;Bjork and Bjork, 2009;Borragán et al, 2017;Just and Carpenter, 1992;Logie, 2011;Ma et al, 2014;Popov et al, 2019;Vergauwe and Cowan, 2015). This resource limit can occur during encoding, such as when the amount of processing resource cannot cope with task demand (Camos and Portrat, 2015); consolidation, such as when multiple representations are competing for storage resources (McFarlane and Humphreys, 2012;Zhang and Luck, 2008); or retrieval, such as when previously retrieved information interferes with the ongoing retrieval process (Wixted and Rohrer, 1993).…”
Section: How Effective Is Caffeine As a Memory Enhancer?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some studies have analogised cognitive resources to muscle strength, which depletes with sustained use and recovers over time (Popov and Reder, 2020). As muscle strength, stamina, and repair can be promoted by diet or exercise (Maughan, 2002), the amount and availability of cognitive resources may also be enhanced through behavioural or pharmacological interventions (Popov et al, 2019;Popov and Reder, 2020). Existing evidence suggests that in healthy adults, sleeping, physical activities, noninvasive brain stimulation, and nootropics can be applied to boost global cognitive functions (Boggio et al, 2009;Manenti et al, 2013).…”
Section: How Effective Is Caffeine As a Memory Enhancer?mentioning
confidence: 99%