2013
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00493
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

fMRI orientation decoding in V1 does not require global maps or globally coherent orientation stimuli

Abstract: The orientation of a large grating can be decoded from V1 functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data, even at low resolution (3-mm isotropic voxels). This finding has suggested that columnar-level neuronal information might be accessible to fMRI at 3T. However, orientation decodability might alternatively arise from global orientation-preference maps. Such global maps across V1 could result from bottom-up processing, if the preferences of V1 neurons were biased toward particular orientations (e.g., radi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

22
118
3

Year Published

2014
2014
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 70 publications
(143 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
22
118
3
Order By: Relevance
“…The response is broadband. This is in agreement with studies showing that information for orientation decoding is not strongly localized to either low or high spatial frequency patterns of activation in the cortex, which are the predictions of the global map and hyperacuity accounts respectively (Kamitani and Sawahata, 2010;Kriegeskorte et al, 2010;Op de Beeck, 2010;Swisher et al, 2010;Freeman et al, 2011;Alink et al, 2013), albeit this is somewhat oversimplified.…”
Section: Edges-related Activity Masquerades As a Radial Biassupporting
confidence: 87%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The response is broadband. This is in agreement with studies showing that information for orientation decoding is not strongly localized to either low or high spatial frequency patterns of activation in the cortex, which are the predictions of the global map and hyperacuity accounts respectively (Kamitani and Sawahata, 2010;Kriegeskorte et al, 2010;Op de Beeck, 2010;Swisher et al, 2010;Freeman et al, 2011;Alink et al, 2013), albeit this is somewhat oversimplified.…”
Section: Edges-related Activity Masquerades As a Radial Biassupporting
confidence: 87%
“…The results, however, have been equivocal (Kamitani and Sawahata, 2010;Kriegeskorte et al, 2010;Swisher et al, 2010;Alink et al, 2013). Freeman et al (2011) attempted to establish what information is "necessary" and "sufficient" for decoding; but again the findings were not so clear-cut (Alink et al, 2013). Finally there was a stimulus-based approach, spurred by the argument that radially balanced spirals should not be decodable if decoding relies solely on a radial bias, but this again was not definitive (Mannion et al, 2009;Freeman et al, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Visual gratings were chosen as stimuli because their encoding in visual cortical response patterns is reasonably well understood. In particular, we examined the accuracy of multivariate decoding in the primary visual cortex (V1), which is generally high (Alink, Krugliak, Walther, & Kriegeskorte, 2013; Kamitani & Tong, 2005; Tong et al, 2010). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, neurons responding to radial orientations with respect to the fixation point appear to be more frequent, creating a global areal map of radial orientation frequencies (Freeman, Heeger, & Merriam, 2013; Sasaki et al, 2006). Neurons responding independently of radial bias are organized in a much finer‐grained columnar map of orientation preference (Alink et al, 2013; Swindale, Grinvald, & Shmuel, 2003; Tong et al, 2010). The different spatial frequencies of these two nested organizations leads us to expect that that the effectiveness of PMC may vary with the spatial scale of the fMRI measurement, and with the visual field coverage of a given region of interest.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%