2016
DOI: 10.1002/icd.1977
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Five‐Month‐old Infants' Discrimination of Visual–Tactile Synchronous Facial Stimulation

Abstract: The distinction between self and other is crucial for self‐awareness and for our awareness of others. However, how human beings learn to associate the face they see in the mirror with themselves is still a matter of debate. The exploration of body‐related multisensory processing with infants has demonstrated that they can detect visual‐tactile contingencies, suggesting the presence of early implicit body perception simply based on the spatiotemporal matching between visual and tactile stimuli alone. In the pre… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
17
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
(33 reference statements)
2
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Another possibility is that infants found the stranger so novel that they attended to processing the identity of her face at the expense of attending to differences in stimuli synchronicity. Although we cannot exclude this possibility based on the current data, it seems unlikely, because participants in similar tasks discriminated between two videos of an unfamiliar infant's face on the basis of synchronicity (Filippetti et al, 2013(Filippetti et al, , 2016. However, wariness of strangers is known to increase around this age (Sroufe, 1977), and future work can address this explanation by adding a condition with a familiarized stranger, such as a researcher who interacts with the infant before the experiment.…”
Section: Mibo Task Looking Preferencesmentioning
confidence: 85%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Another possibility is that infants found the stranger so novel that they attended to processing the identity of her face at the expense of attending to differences in stimuli synchronicity. Although we cannot exclude this possibility based on the current data, it seems unlikely, because participants in similar tasks discriminated between two videos of an unfamiliar infant's face on the basis of synchronicity (Filippetti et al, 2013(Filippetti et al, , 2016. However, wariness of strangers is known to increase around this age (Sroufe, 1977), and future work can address this explanation by adding a condition with a familiarized stranger, such as a researcher who interacts with the infant before the experiment.…”
Section: Mibo Task Looking Preferencesmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…We chose this age because infants have been shown to prefer synchrony to asynchrony in visual-tactile tasks at 5, 7, and 10 months of age (Filippetti et al, 2016;Zmyj et al, 2011) and because of the abundance of research into face-to-face parent-infant interactions in the second half of the first year (Feldman, 2007). We based a priori sample size on effect sizes calculated from published data for visual-tactile synchrony effects in newborns (Filippetti et al, 2013) and 5-month-olds (Filippetti et al, 2016). A sample of 16 yields 95% power to detect a medium effect size of 0.5 in a 2 9 2 within-participants design, according to GPower (version 3.1.9.2; Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007).…”
Section: Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Then, a synchronous preference score was calculated considering the looking time to the synchronous video over the total looking time to the screen. This score was calculated on the looking time instead of the total time of stimulus presentation (12 s for each trial) in order to take into account the effective interest of the infant during each trial (Turati et al, 2005;Filippetti et al, 2016). The same experimenter coded all the videos using the Datavyu software, a video coding and data visualization tool for collecting behavioral data from video (Datavyu Team, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Considering the neurophysiological characteristics of affective touch and evidence from adult research (Crucianelli et al, 2013;van Stralen et al, 2014;Panagiotopoulou et al, 2017), in the present study we aimed to investigate whether affective touch may promote implicit bodily self-awareness in early infancy, by facilitating the detection of body-related visual-tactile contingencies. More specifically, we built on a previous visual preference paradigm developed by Filippetti et al (2016). In this study, 5-month-old infants were presented with two sideby-side videos of a baby's face being stroked on the cheek.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Developmentally, a large set of studies have indeed demonstrated infants' early abilities to discriminate visual‐proprioceptive contingency arising from their own movements (Bahrick & Watson, ; Morgan & Rochat, ; Reddy, Chisholm, Forrester, Conforti, & Maniatopoulou, ; Rochat & Morgan, ; Schmuckler & Jewell, ; Watson, ), as well as perfectly synchronous multisensory cues related to the body (Filippetti, Farroni, & Johnson, ; Filippetti, Johnson, Lloyd‐Fox, Dragovic, & Farroni, ; Zmyj, Jank, Schütz‐Bosbach, & Daum, ). However, while these studies show that multisensory contingency becomes functional quite early in life, the ability to discriminate this information does not necessarily imply that the infant is able to recognize these movements and body parts as belonging to the self (Bremner, Holmes, & Spence, ; Lewis & Brooks‐Gunn, ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%